Waistlines and Ratings of Executives: Does Executive Status Overcome Obesity Stigma?

AuthorLinda R. Shanock,Eden B. King,Steven G. Rogelberg,Phillip W. Braddy,Sharon C. Doerer,Sharon McDowell‐Larsen,Michelle R. Hebl
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1002/hrm.21667
Published date01 March 2016
Date01 March 2016
Human Resource Management, March–April 2016, Vol. 55, No. 2. Pp. 283–300
© 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Published online in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com).
DOI:10.1002/hrm.21667
Correspondence to: Eden B. King, George Mason University, Department of Psychology, 4400 University Drive
MSN3f5, Fairfax, Virginia 22030, Phone: 703-993-1620, Fax: 713-993-1620, E-mail: eking6@gmu.edu
WAISTLINES AND RATINGS OF
EXECUTIVES: DOES EXECUTIVE
STATUS OVERCOME OBESITY STIGMA?
EDEN B. KING, STEVEN G. ROGELBERG, MICHELLE R. HEBL,
PHILLIP W. BRADDY, LINDA R. SHANOCK,
SHARON C. DOERER, AND SHARON MCDOWELL-LARSEN
Top executives hold positions that convey power, prestige, and competence as
predicted by status characteristics theory. Nevertheless, the impressions gener-
ated through this status characteristic may be vulnerable when executives also
possess characteristics that refl ect a devalued social identity, such as obesity. Data
from health examinations and multisource evaluation surveys of 757 CEOs, vice
presidents, and upper managers suggest that the observable cue of umbilical
waist circumference is negatively associated with evaluations of leaders across
hierarchical levels, even after controlling for Body Mass Index, physical activity,
personality, and demographic characteristics. Thus, hierarchically based status
characteristics are insuffi cient in overcoming the stigma of obesity: even CEOs are
subject to the pernicious effects of obesity stigma. © 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Keywords: diversity, social issues
A
few months after devouring doughnuts
on the popular Late Show with David
Letterman, admittedly obese New Jersey
Governor Chris Christie confirmed that
he underwent gastric band surgery. The
Wall Street Journal described this decision as a
“move that comes amid concerns about his
health as he emerges as a national player in the
Republican Party” (Haddon & Winslow, 2013).
Beyond physical health concerns, party strategists
may also be aware that obesity is associated with
negative interpersonal perceptions that might
impede political success.
According to the National Institutes of
Health (NIH), two thirds of the American popula-
tion is overweight or obese (NIH, 2010). Despite
the prevalence of obesity, Americans’ attitudes
toward and perceptions of heavy individuals are
typically more negative than those toward other
targets of stigma (Crandall & Martinez, 1996).
Whereas members of many social identity groups
derive positive self-image and support from their
in-groups, even the parents of obese children
(Crandall, 1994) and obese individuals themselves
often stigmatize obesity (Crandall & Biernat, 1990;
Crocker, Cornwell, & Major, 1993). Heavy individ-
uals are evaluated more negatively than thin indi-
viduals in both selection (Finkelstein, Demuth, &
Sweeney, 2007) and training contexts (Shapiro,
King, & Quiñones, 2007), and increases in weight
are associated with decreases in income (Judge
& Cable, 2011). Indeed, reviews of evidence on
employment discrimination toward obese work-
ers yield similar conclusions of general stigmatiza-
tion (Roehling, 1999; Rudolph, Wells, Weller, &
Baltes, 2008). However, the extent to which such
284 HUMAN RESOURCE MANAGEMENT, MARCH–APRIL 2016
Human Resource Management DOI: 10.1002/hrm
The negative status
associated with the
stigma of obesity
may overshadow
the powerful status
characteristic of a top
management position.
high status positions. Fourth, studies outside
of the laboratory rely nearly exclusively on self-
report survey data or perceived discrimination
(Roehling, 1999), which is subject to perceptual
biases (Crosby, 1984; Elgar, Roberts, Tudor-Smith,
& Moore, 2005). Fifth, while field studies typi-
cally control for or explore the role of gender and
ethnicity on weight discrimination, they rarely
include other characteristics (such as personality)
that may influence indices of bias (M. V. Roehling,
Roehling, & Odland, 2008). Sixth, both laboratory
and field studies are limited in their operation-
alization of obesity by relying on judgments of
“overweight” versus “not overweight” body sizes
as a function of photographs or body mass index
(BMI) that may not fully account for cues that give
rise to the stigma of obesity. Finally, nearly every
study previously conducted on the stigma of obe-
sity has focused on employees in or applicants for
low- to midlevel positions. Thus, while it is clear
that fictitious individuals who appear to be obese
are stigmatized in low- and midlevel jobs, it is not
yet known whether individuals at the highest lev-
els of organizations are subjected to bias.
These limitations, taken with the predictions of
status characteristics theory, give rise to questions
about the boundary conditions and generalizabil-
ity of obesity discrimination in the workplace.
The central question addressed in this paper is,
does the stigma of obesity eclipse the positive
outcomes resulting from status associated with
a top management position? Thus, this research
makes four contributions to the literature. First,
we uniquely integrate sociological and psycholog-
ical perspectives by drawing from status character-
istics and stigma theories to understand whether
devalued physical characteristics can overwhelm
positive hierarchical status cues. Second, by exam-
ining multisource ratings of actual top executives
(rather than self-ratings, evaluations of strangers,
or “paper-people” in a lab setting) and upper man-
agers, we offer novel empirical evidence address-
ing the question of whether bias affects those
who have attained hierarchical status using raters
who have existing relationships with the execu-
tives. Third, we build understanding of the cues
that give rise to stigma by considering umbili-
cal waist circumference, which was assessed by a
third-party health care professional as a specific,
observable cue that triggers negative expectations.
We also test the robustness of our findings by con-
sidering additional factors—personality, physical
activity, and demographic characteristics—that
could impinge on the relationship between upper
managers’ weight and their performance ratings.
Fourth, in considering obesity stigma as it inter-
sects with age and gender, this work addresses
findings extend to the highest levels—one that
conveys substantial social status—is unclear.
According to status characteristics theory, val-
ued attributes (i.e., status characteristics) give rise
to positive performance expectations and impres-
sions of competence (Berger, Cohen, & Zelditch,
1966). In addition, positive interpretations of
behavior can be associated with status charac-
teristics that are valued (Gerber, 1996). Being a
member of the C-suite, much like occupying a
high-level political office, conveys wealth, power,
prestige, and competence (Ravlin & Thomas,
2005). Indeed, leaders have greater influence and
are rated more positively with regard to power and
competence than are lower status group members
(Harvey, 1953; Hollander, 1961; Sherif, White, &
Harvey, 1955; Torrance, 1955). Though expecta-
tions of individuals are typically derived from
more than a single characteristic (Humphreys &
Berger, 1981), it is difficult to conceive of charac-
teristics that reduce the status conveyed by a top
executive job title in the workplace. Nevertheless,
these positive expectations and
impressions of competence may be
vulnerable when top executives also
possess characteristics that reflect
a devalued (i.e., stigmatized) social
identity—especially when the deval-
ued attribute is highly visible as in
the case of obesity.
We propose that the negative
status associated with the stigma of
obesity may overshadow the pow-
erful status characteristic of a top
management position, considered
here as CEOs or company presi-
dents. This study addresses meaningful internal
and external validity limitations of the existing
body of research on obesity discrimination in the
workplace (Puhl & Heuer, 2009; Rudolph et al.,
2008). First, conclusions about employment dis-
crimination toward obese individuals are primar-
ily based on experiments that assess bias toward
fictitious targets (i.e., “paper-people”; see Rudolph
et al., 2008) that can inflate relationships found
in authentic samples (see Murphy, Herr, Lockhart,
& Maguire, 1986). Second, laboratory studies gen-
erally fail to account for preexisting relationships
that occur between raters and ratees in real-world
settings, raising questions about the generaliz-
ability of existing findings in light of evidence
that biases are less likely to emerge when raters
know more about targets (Fiske & Neuberg, 1990).
Third, these laboratory studies tend to focus on
analyses contrasting narrow job types, such as
positions involving high and low levels of con-
tact with the public, and thus may not apply to

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT