Performance benefits of reward choice: a procedural justice perspective

DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/1748-8583.12073
Published date01 April 2015
AuthorGregory B. Northcraft,Arran Caza,Matthew W. McCarter
Date01 April 2015
Performance benefits of reward choice: a
procedural justice perspective
Arran Caza, Griffith University and University of Manitoba
Matthew W. McCarter, University of Texas at San Antonio and Chapman
University
Gregory B. Northcraft, University of Illinois
Human Resource Management Journal, Vol 25, no 2, 2015, pages 184–199
Reward choice – employees’ ability to exercise control over the formal rewards they receive from work
– is an important part of many HRM strategies. Reward choice is expected to increase employee
performance, but conflicting findings highlight the need to better understand how and when it will do
so. Based on fairness heuristic theory, we predicted that procedural justice mediates reward choice’s
influence on performance, and that choice attractiveness moderates that influence. A field study and an
experiment both had similar results, supporting our predictions. Reward choice can increase performance
by as much as 40 per cent, but only when the available choices are attractive to employees.
Contact: Arran Caza, Asper School of Business, Drake Centre, 181 Freedman Cr, Winnipeg,
MB, Canada R3T 5V4. Email: a.caza@griffith.edu.au; arran.caza@umanitoba.ca
Keywords: choice; organisational justice; employee performance; rewards
INTRODUCTION
An increasingly popular HRM strategy is giving employees reward choice, which provides
employees with the ability to choose the level or type of rewards they receive from work
(Miceli and Heneman, 2000; White, 2009; IOMA, 2011). In the US, for example, fewer
than 20 major employers allowed workers any control over how they were rewarded in 1980
(Hewitt Associates, 1993), but almost all major employers were offering some reward choice by
2007 (Employee Benefits, 2007). The use of reward choice in other countries shows the same
increasing trend (Rao, 2008; Koo, 2011). Allowing employees to exercise some control over the
nature of their rewards has become an important part of many organisations’ HRM strategy.
Organisations appear to have embraced reward choice for two reasons: to control costs by
only providing those rewards that employees actually value, and to benefit from improvements
in workers’ attitudes and behaviour (Kelliher and Anderson, 2008; Lovewell, 2010). The use of
choice reflects the fact that the traditional approach of offering standardised rewards has not
succeeded in controlling costs or enhancing performance; such generic reward plans have
tended to produce poor results (Beer and Cannon, 2004; Chiang and Birtch, 2006). Reward
choice allows organisations to provide rewards that are customised to the individual, and is
believed to help when competing to recruit and retain the best employees, as well as
contributing to their subsequent performance (Fay and Thompson, 2001; Koo, 2011; Nazir et al.,
2012). Consistent with the belief that reward choice benefits the worker and the organisation,
scholarship has linked reward choice with both increased task performance and
performance-related attitudes, such as organisational commitment (Lawler and Hackman, 1969;
Cooper et al., 1992; Cole and Flint, 2004).
However, despite the widespread use of reward choice and research evidence of its effect on
performance, two important gaps remain in the literature. The first gap concerns the
bs_bs_banner
doi: 10.1111/1748-8583.12073
HUMAN RESOURCE MANAGEMENT JOURNAL, VOL 25 NO 2, 2015184
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Please cite this article in press as: Caza, A., McCarter, M.W. and Northcraft, G.B. (2015) ‘Performance benefits of reward choice: a procedural justice
perspective’. Human Resource Management Journal 25: 2, 184–199.
mechanism linking reward choice to performance, which has remained unknown. The second
gap concerns anecdotal and empirical evidence suggesting that employers do not always get
a positive response from introducing reward choice (Sullivan, 2009; IOMA, 2011; Shreeram,
2012). While some studies have found a positive relationship between reward choice and
performance (Lawler and Hackman, 1969; Cooper et al., 1992), at least one study (Morgeson
et al., 2001) found that reward choice was not beneficial. It seems that reward choice can
improve employee performance, but does not always do so. As such, the crucial next step for
theory and practice lies in understanding how reward choice affects performance, including the
mediating mechanism and boundary conditions.
In service of this goal, we report two studies that answer the two research questions: What
is the mediator of the relationship between reward choice and work performance? and What
are the boundary conditions of this relationship? Study 1 used a field survey to extend previous
research by showing that reward choice influences performance-related attitudes through the
mediating mechanism of procedural justice. Study 2 used an experimental design to replicate
the Study 1 findings and to examine boundary conditions for reward choice. Study 2 found that
reward attractiveness is an important moderator of the effect of reward choice (i.e. reward
choice only increases justice and performance if the available choices are attractive).
THEORETICAL BACKGROUND AND HYPOTHESES
To study the relationship between reward choice and performance, we used the total rewards
perspective (Nazir et al., 2012; Lin et al., 2014). This perspective defines rewards to include all
of the valued outcomes that employees derive from their work, including base pay, incentives,
non-salary benefits and perquisites (Fay and Thompson, 2001; Gross and Friedman, 2004;
Chiang and Birtch, 2006). Focusing on an employee’s total rewards is consistent with larger
trends in the study of HRM, which are recognising the need to move towards more holistic
views (Boxall, 2013). The total rewards perspective is based on the fact that different employees
value different types of rewards (Kinnie et al., 2005; Krausert, 2014), but their response to
having their desires satisfied will be similar. For example, imagine one worker who desires
more pay, and another who is most concerned with flexible work arrangements. Giving both
workers the same reward (e.g. more pay) would provoke differing responses, but if each worker
is given what s/he desires, s/he should have similarly positive responses (Cable and Edwards,
2004). The total rewards perspective focuses on compensation practices in general, so as to
accommodate such diversity in the workforce.
Reward choice and performance
Organisations are increasingly offering customised total rewards by allowing workers to control
some aspects of the level or type of rewards they receive. The specifics of the reward choice
vary from simple forms such as choosing among different package options to situations where
an employee’s entire reward package is idiosyncratic (e.g. Anand et al., 2010). Providing
employees with reward choice is expected to reduce organisational costs, increase worker
satisfaction and improve worker performance (Kelliher and Anderson, 2008; Lovewell, 2010).
The first two effects of reward choice – reduced cost and increased reward satisfaction – are
straightforward. It has been documented that introducing reward choice decreases the cost of
reward provision while simultaneously increasing workers’ satisfaction (Karoly and Panis,
2004; Dencker et al., 2007).
Beyond these two effects, however, there is some evidence that reward choice can also
improve performance. For example, one study found that workers who designed their own
Arran Caza, Matthew W. McCarter and Gregory B. Northcraft
HUMAN RESOURCE MANAGEMENT JOURNAL, VOL 25 NO 2, 2015 185
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT