Is Perceived Creativity‐Reward Contingency Good for Creativity? The Role of Challenge and Threat Appraisals

AuthorKwok Leung,Hong Deng,Yang Zhao,Fuli Li
Date01 July 2017
Published date01 July 2017
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1002/hrm.21795
Human Resource Management, July–August 2017, Vol. 56, No. 4. Pp. 693–709
© 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Published online in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com).
DOI:10.1002/hrm.21795
Correspondence to: Hong Deng, Manchester Business School, Booth Street West, Manchester, M15 6PB, UK,
Phone:(44) 0161 820 8343, E-mail: xhxghome@gmail.com.
facilitate creativity (Zhou & Oldham, 2001; Zhou
& Shalley, 2003). The use of extrinsic rewards
such as monetary rewards and management rec-
ognition (Fairbank & Williams, 2001; van Dijk &
van den Ende, 2002) has a long history in orga-
nizations (Klotz, Wheeler, Halbesleben, Brock, &
Buckley, 2011; Zhou & Shalley, 2003), and reward
programs are commonly used to spur creative
performance.
Employee creativity is crucial to organiza-
tional growth and its effectiveness and sur-
vival (Kanter, 1983; Nonaka, 1991). When
employees exhibit workplace creativity,
they generate novel and potentially useful
ideas concerning organizational products, prac-
tices, services, or procedures (Shalley & Gilson,
2004). There has been considerable interest in
understanding which management practices can
IS PERCEIVED CREATIVITY-
REWARD CONTINGENCY GOOD
FOR CREATIVITY? THE ROLE
OF CHALLENGE AND THREAT
APPRAISALS
FULI LI, HONG DENG, KWOK LEUNG, AND YANG ZHAO
To address the complex effect of perceived reward for creativity on creative
performance, we examined the role of cognitive appraisal as an individual dif-
ference variable. An individual’s appraisal of reward for creativity, including chal-
lenge appraisal (perceived potential for recognition, growth, or mastery) and
threat appraisal (perceived potential for revealing incompetence and damaging
self-respect), is hypothesized to shape the effects of perceived reward for creativ-
ity. We further expect creativity-related intrinsic motivation to play a mediating
role in the perceived reward-creativity relationship. The results of a three-wave
eld study showed that when challenge appraisal was high, perceived reward
was positively related to creative performance through creativity-related intrin-
sic motivation, whereas when threat appraisal was high, perceived reward was
negatively related to creative performance through creativity-related intrinsic
motivation. A similar analysis showed that intrinsic task motivation was not able
to channel the moderating effect of perceived reward and individual appraisal on
creative performance. ©2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Keywords: creativity, perception, research methods and design–quantitative
research methodology
694 HUMAN RESOURCE MANAGEMENT, JULY–AUGUST 2017
Human Resource Management DOI: 10.1002/hrm
Despite the popularity
of the use of extrinsic
rewards to drive
creativity, conclusive
evidence is not yet
available from field
studies concerning
the effects of
extrinsic rewards on
creativity.
1996; Woodman, Sawyer, & Griffin, 1993), which
emphasizes the joint effects of individual dif-
ferences and rewards, a variable derived from
the context. Drawing on this perspective, this
research aims to develop a theoretical framework
with a focus on how individuals’ characteristics
account for the mixed relationship between per-
ceived reward and creativity via intrinsic motiva-
tion. In spite of the divergence between the two
camps of researchers with different theoretical
orientations, they converge on the critical role of
competence assessment. Both camps agree that if
extrinsic rewards are perceived to convey positive
information about one’s competence, they would
increase intrinsic motivation (e.g., Deci, Koestner,
& Ryan, 1999; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Eisenberger,
1992; Eisenberger & Armeli, 1997). The individual
difference perspective suggests that the perceived
meaning of reward for creativity is important in
determining its effect on intrinsic motivation and,
hence, creativity. Reward for creativity may imbue
a variety of meanings and can be perceived as
providing an opportunity to acquire recognition
or appreciation, respect and personal develop-
ment (cf. the reflection theory of compensation;
Thierry, 2001). We base our theorizing on the well-
established transactional theory of stress (Lazarus
& Folkman, 1984) because contexts characterized
by social evaluation constitute a type of psycho-
logical stressor (cf. Byron, Khazanchi, & Nazarian,
2010). Reward for creativity involves social evalu-
ation and can be viewed as a potentially psycho-
logical stressor. In fact, research has associated
performance-contingent reward with stress-induc-
ing conditions such as time pressure, continuous
monitoring of performance, and uncertainty of
outcomes (Shirom, Westman, & Melamed, 1999)
and found it to be stressful (Landy, Quick, & Kasl,
1994).
The conceptualization of reward for creativ-
ity as a stressor makes the transactional theory of
stress highly relevant for analyzing its effects on
creativity. Drawing on this theoretical framework,
we introduce the notion of cognitive appraisal to
the reward context and propose that individual
appraisal of the meaning of reward for creativity
is a key individual difference that influences the
responses to such a reward. Given the fact that fail-
ure is likely in creative endeavors (Harackiewicz &
Sansone, 2000), the contingent reward for creativ-
ity constitutes as much a threat to lose as a chance
to win. The fear of failure would make such
reward threatening even if it is designed to convey
positive competence information (Harackiewicz &
Sansone, 2000). The major objective of the pres-
ent research is to develop a person-reward inter-
action model that examines how individuals’
Despite the popularity of the use of extrinsic
rewards to drive creativity, conclusive evidence
is not yet available from field studies concerning
the effects of extrinsic rewards on creativity. A
number of field studies have reported a nonsig-
nificant relationship between perceived reward
for creativity and employee creativity (e.g., Baer,
Oldham, & Cummings, 2003; Dewett, 2002; Eder,
2007; George & Zhou, 2002; Yoon, Choi, Lee, &
Kim, 2009). As a result, Zhou and Shalley (2003,
pp. 204–205) have urged researchers to “system-
atically untangle the complexity of the impact of
rewards on creativity in the workplace.”
To address this issue, it is important to exam-
ine the process underlying the relation between
perceived reward and intrinsic motivation (Baer
et al., 2003), which is a key driver of creativity
(Amabile, 1979; Shin & Zhou, 2003). Intrinsic
motivation, which refers to “motivation that
arises from the individual’s positive
reaction to qualities of the task itself;
this reaction can be experienced
as interest, involvement, curiosity,
satisfaction or positive challenge”
(Amabile, 1996, p. 115), has long
been argued as a pivotal mediator
that channels the effects of anteced-
ent variables on creativity (Amabile,
1988, 1996). In fact, the debate on
the effects of extrinsic rewards on
creativity is essentially centered on
its relationship with intrinsic moti-
vation. Researchers with a social-
cognitive orientation argue that
extrinsic rewards reduce intrinsic
motivation and creativity due to
lowered self-determination and the
overjustification effect (Amabile,
1996; Hennessey & Amabile, 1988). In contrast,
behaviorally oriented researchers argue that extrin-
sic rewards increase perceived self-determination,
thus facilitating intrinsic motivation (Eisenberger
& Cameron, 1996; Eisenberger, Pierce & Cameron,
1999) and creativity (Eisenberger & Aselage, 2009).
This debate has not been settled as empirical
field evidence is rather mixed. For instance, Eden
(1975) reported a negative correlation between
the perception of extrinsic rewards and intrinsic
motivation, but Eisenberger and Aselage (2009)
found a positive correlation between them.
Shalley, Zhou, and Oldham (2004) suggested
that the role of individual differences should be
considered because individuals with different
characteristics or orientations may respond differ-
ently to extrinsic rewards. Their proposal echoes
the view of person-context interaction (e.g.,
George & Zhou, 2001; Oldham & Cummings,

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT