What Makes a Good Trader? On the Role of Intuition and Reflection on Trader Performance

DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/jofi.12619
Published date01 June 2018
AuthorMARK DESANTIS,DAVID PORTER,BRICE CORGNET
Date01 June 2018
THE JOURNAL OF FINANCE VOL. LXXIII, NO. 3 JUNE 2018
What Makes a Good Trader? On the Role of
Intuition and Reflection on Trader Performance
BRICE CORGNET, MARK DESANTIS, and DAVID PORTER
ABSTRACT
Using laboratory experiments, we provide evidence on three factors influencing trader
performance: fluid intelligence, cognitive reflection, and theory of mind (ToM). Fluid
intelligence provides traders with computational skills necessary to draw a statisti-
cal inference. Cognitive reflection helps traders avoid behavioral biases and thereby
extract signals from market orders and update their prior beliefs accordingly. ToM
describes the degree to which traders correctly assess the informational content of or-
ders. We show that cognitive reflection and ToMare complementary because traders
benefit from understanding signals’ quality only if they are capable of processing
these signals.
THE CORNERSTONE MODELS OF FINANCE build on the assumption that a represen-
tative economic agent acts rationally (e.g., Markowitz (1952), Sharpe (1964),
Samuelson (1970)). However, the rationality assumption, which requires that
individuals correctly apply and carry out mathematical and statistical meth-
ods, has been challenged by behavioral finance studies that find a large degree
of variability in individuals’ ability to solve complex problems. When presented
with such problems, most individuals tend to rely on simple heuristics instead
of performing the requisite calculations (e.g., see Thaler (1993,2005), Barberis
and Thaler (2003), Shefrin (2007) for surveys).
The heterogeneity in individuals’ cognitive capacities suggests that we may
observe significant differences in their financial decisions. Broader access to the
stock market coupled with increasing complexity of the financial environment
(e.g., large number of sophisticated financial instruments, interconnectedness
of global markets, etc.) thus renders understanding of the relationship be-
tween individuals’ cognitive capacities and financial decisions of great practical
importance.
Different factors influencing trader performance have been studied in isola-
tion in different strands of the literature using various types of data (archival,
Brice Corgnet is with EMLYONBusiness School, GATE L-SE. Mark DeSantis and David Porter
are with Chapman University. We thank the Editor, an anonymous Associate Editor, and three
anonymous referees for insightful and constructive comments. Brice Corgnet also acknowledges
the support of LABEX CORTEX. We obtained Institutional Review Board approval to perform our
experiment and collect data on human subjects. We do not have any sources of financial support to
disclose.
DOI: 10.1111/jofi.12619
1113
1114 The Journal of Finance R
experimental, and neuroimaging) and various measures of performance, such
as forecasting skill (Bruguier, Quartz, and Bossaerts (2010); BQB henceforth)
or stock-picking skill (Grinblatt, Keloharju, and Linnainmaa (2012)). In this
paper, we assess the cognitive underpinnings of trading decisions using a uni-
fied market setting. Todo so, we employ an experimental asset market in which
we control various aspects of the trading environment (see Bossaerts (2009),
Frydman et al. (2014), Noussair and Tucker (2014)). Specifically, we manage
the flow of information into the market so that the effect of an individual’s skill
is not confounded with that of insider trading. This is the case because the
experimental method allows us to randomize the distribution of information
across traders so that traders exhibiting high levels of specific skills are, on
average, just as informed as other traders. This market setting allows us to
control the flow of private information by allocating a specific signal regarding
the true value of the traded asset to each trader (see Plott and Sunder (1988);
PS henceforth).
Our experimental methodology also allows us to collect a large set of char-
acteristics on individual traders. Controlling for an extensive number of
individual-level characteristics (such as financial literacy, personality traits,
educational background, and risk attitudes) in addition to the flow of infor-
mation in the market ensures the robustness of the effect of our conjectured
predictors.
We posit that traders’ ability to learn the true value of an asset using both
private information and market orders is a key to traders’ success. All else
equal, a more informed trader will ultimately post more profitable orders,
which results in higher earnings. In the extreme, a trader who knows an asset’s
true value will be assured of earning positive trading profits by systematically
buying below and selling above the true value. Thus, to ensure positive trading
profits, a trader should attempt to discover the asset’s true value. Weargue that
learning the true value of an asset requires three fundamental skills, namely,
the ability to conduct statistical inference, avoid behavioral biases, and assess
the informational content of market signals.
Statistical inference based on available signals is crucial for traders to
learn the true value of an asset. Such calculations require “fluid intelli-
gence,” which is defined as one’s ability to conduct mental simulations and
engage in abstract reasoning (e.g., Mackintosh (2011)). In short, fluid intelli-
gence is the computational capacity in which one needs to undertake mental
calculations.
In addition to fluid intelligence, “cognitive reflection” is a crucial skill for
traders to learn true asset value. Cognitive reflection is defined as one’s ability
to engage in effortful reasoning (e.g., Stanovich (2009)) and is measured using
the cognitive reflection test (CRT), which consists of questions that have an
intuitive but incorrect answer (e.g., “A bat and a ball cost $1.10 in total. The
bat costs a dollar more than the ball. How much does the ball cost?” Frederick
(2005)). Upon reflection, one may disregard the intuitive answer (“10¢”) in
favor of the correct one (“5¢”). Cognitive reflection adds to fluid intelligence
because it helps individuals avoid commonly observed heuristics and biases

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT