The sound of cooperation: Musical influences on cooperative behavior

DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1002/job.2128
AuthorBrian Wansink,Kevin M. Kniffin,William D. Schulze,Jubo Yan
Published date01 March 2017
Date01 March 2017
The sound of cooperation: Musical inuences on
cooperative behavior
KEVIN M. KNIFFIN
1
*,JUBOYAN
2
, BRIAN WANSINK
1
AND WILLIAM D. SCHULZE
1
1
Cornell University, Dyson School of Applied Economics and Management, New York, U.S.A.
2
Division of Economics, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
Summary Music as an environmental aspect of professional workplaces has been closely studied with respect to
consumer behavior while sparse attention has been given to its relevance for employee behavior. In this arti-
cle, we focus on the inuence of music upon cooperative behavior within decision-making groups. Based on
results from two extended 20-round public goods experiments, we nd that happy music signicantly and
positively inuences cooperative behavior. We also nd a signicant positive association between mood
and cooperative behavior. Consequently, while our studies provide partial support for the relevance of affect
in relation to cooperation within groups, we also show an independently important function of happy music
that ts with a theory of synchronous and rhythmic activity as a social lubricant. More generally, our ndings
indicate that music and perhaps other atmospheric variables that are designed to prime consumer behavior
might have comparably important effects for employees and consequently warrant closer investigation.
Copyright © 2016 The Authors Journal of Organizational Behavior Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Keywords: music; cooperation; experimental economics; consumer behavior; organizational behavior
Musical resources are drawn upon for many uses. In retail establishments, for example, store managers and adver-
tisers are careful to play songs with the goal of encouraging behaviors and attitudes that are more likely to result in
greater sales (e.g., Munichor & Rafaeli, 2007; North & Hargreaves, 1998, 2003; North, Hargreaves, & McKendrick,
2000; Strick et al., 2015; Wansink & van Ittersum, 2012; Yalch, 1991). In sports, athletes tend to demonstrate
greater performance while reporting less exertion when competing with their favorite music (e.g., Karageorgh is &
Priest, 2012). And, in romantic relationships, the phrase mood musicreects a tradition of suitors using music
to woo the subjects of their affection (e.g., Miller, 2000).
In the same way that there are myriad reasons why people listen to music (e.g., Tarrant & No rth, 2000, 2001), it is
also clear that people do so in a wide variety of environmental settings. Rentfrow and Gosling (2003), for example,
report that people commonly listen to music when they are driving, reading, exercising, spending time with friends,
and when they are alone. Indeed, the pervasiveness with which music is played appears to contribute to it being
taken-for-granted(Suddaby et al., 2010) as part of the ambient environment. In our case, we are interested in
the potential for music to signicantly inuence cooperative behavior. Our interests are complementary to recent
ndings that show the importance of an organizations physical features for facilitating cooperative behavior
(e.g., Ashkanasy et al., 2014; Knifn et al., 2015).
Among academic researchers, music has been closely studied for its marketing value in relation to its potential
inuence upon consumer behavior (e.g., Bailey & Areni, 2006; Hui, Dube, & Chebat, 1997; Morin, Dube, &
Chebat, 2007); however, there has been sparse attention paid to the relevance of music for employee behavior
and managerial decision making. For example, a search of the subject indices for 10 popular Organizational
Behavior (OB) textbooks yields no entries for music,”“musical,or song(Champoux, 2011; Colquitt et al.,
2009; Gander, Haberberg, & Rieple, 2007; George & Jones, 2008; Grifn & Moorhead, 2007; Hitt et al., 2011;
*Correspondence to: Kevin M. Knifn, Cornell University, Dyson School of Applied Economics and Management, Warren Hall 111, Ithaca, New
York 14853, U.S.A. E-mail: kmk276@cornell.edu
Copyright © 2016 The Authors Journal of Organizational Behavior
Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Received 24 March 2015
Revised 13 June 2016, Accepted 13 July 2016
Journal of Organizational Behavior, J. Organiz. Behav. 38, 372390 (2017)
Published online 9 August 2016 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/job.2128
Research Article
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Johns & Saks, 2001; Kinicki & Kreitner, 2008; Luthans, 2008; McShane & Von Glinow, 2008; Robbins & Judge,
2011). Similarly, a keyword search within a sample of high-prole OB journals for musicand musicalgenerates
no results for the Academy of Management Journal while articles in the Journal of Organizational Behavior, Acad-
emy of Management Review, and Administrative Science Quarterly focus on music as an industry or activity (Albert
& Bell, 2002; Bougon et al., 1977; Dobrow, 2013; Faulkner & Runde, 2009; Greve, 1996; Hackman, 2003; Hirsch,
1975; Lingo & OMahony, 2010; Miner et al., 2001; Moorman & Miner, 1998; Peterson & Berger, 1971) instead of
considering music as a potential inuence on contemporary workplaces. In a partial exception from that pattern,
Fritz et al. (2010) neither focus on music nor present empirical ndings that involve music but they do nevertheless
acknowledge that employees often listen to music on the weekends as one among many tools for recovering from the
stresses of a regular workweek.
In this article, we review previous research concerning musical inuences upon behavior, and we report the results
of two lab experiments that test for the specicinuence of music upon cooperative behavior. As with other stimuli
that are designed to subtly inuence behavior, our experiments are partly motivated by the fact that music is a work-
place feature that can be modied with relative ease and low cost. Indeed, while many retail establishments care-
fully select musical programs that are designed to inuence consumersbehavior during shopping visits (Wansink
& van Ittersum, 2012), it is notable that the employees in those same workplaces are concurrently exposed to the
companys musical selections during their shifts. We focus on studying cooperation given ample evidence that
workplace teams tend to benet when co-workers cooperate with each other (e.g., Grant, 2013; Wilson & Knifn,
2003). With specic respect to music, our interests are responsive to recent calls (e.g., Ashkanasy, Ayoko, &
Jehn, 2014; Elsbach & Pratt, 2007) to examine ways in which ofce atmospherics might inuence employee
behavior.
Our studies draw upon two main theoretical frameworks that relate to understanding the relevance of environmen-
tal features on individual decision making within groups. Studies 1 and 2 each engage Dynamic Attending Theory
(DAT) (Escofer et al., 2010; Jones & Boltz, 1989; Large & Jones, 1999) to examine whether the synchronous
activity of listening to rhythmic music will contribute signicantly to cooperation while Study 2 applies
Affective Events Theory (AET) (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996) to test the question of whether moodinduced
by happy musicis signicantly and positive associated with prosocial decisions. In their original introduction
of AET, Weiss and Cropanzano (1996, p. 40) catalogue a long list of environmental factors such as tempera-
ture, humidity, pollutants, and crowding that researchers have shown as important for inuencing mood;
however, our focus on happy, rhythmic music offers a chance to concurrently consider AET alongside DAT.
More specically, the intensive laboratory experiments that we present are designed to approximate workplace
environments in order to help us understand (i) whether happy music facilitates cooperative behavior among
co-workers and (ii) the extent to which (if any) the induction of positive mood might explain away the rele-
vance of the music.
Theoretical Background and Hypothesis Development
The physical design of professional workplaces has been understudied within OB (Johns, 2010). In their review of
the topic, Elsbach and Pratt (2007) reported mixed results while generally calling for more attention from
researchers. More recently, Ashkanasy et al. (2014) applied the framework of AET to consider the various ways
in which the physical space of workplaces can interact with social relationships. In their case, Ashkanasy and
co-authors called for researchers to consider questions such as whether working in high-density open-plan
workspaces triggers affective reactions such as anger and frustration that then lead to negative attitudes and poor
work attitudes (low affective commitment)(2014, p. 1180) . In related work, Knifn et al. (2015) highlighted
the specic roles that workplace eateries (e.g., cafeterias, kitchens) can have upon cooperation among co-workers
and team-level performance. Considering the signicant expenses that organizations incur for establishing and
THE SOUND OF COOPERATION 373
Copyright © 2016 The Authors Journal of Organizational Behavior
Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
J. Organiz. Behav. 38, 372390 (2017)
DOI: 10.1002/job

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT