The Performance Effect of Feedback Frequency and Detail: Evidence from a Field Experiment in Customer Satisfaction

DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/1475-679X.12184
Published date01 December 2017
Date01 December 2017
DOI: 10.1111/1475-679X.12184
Journal of Accounting Research
Vol. 55 No. 5 December 2017
Printed in U.S.A.
The Performance Effect of
Feedback Frequency and Detail:
Evidence from a Field Experiment
in Customer Satisfaction
PABLO CASAS-ARCE,
SOFIA M. LOURENC¸O,
AND F. AS´
IS MART´
INEZ-JEREZ
Received 17 June 2015; accepted 11 July 2017
ABSTRACT
This paper presents the results from a field experiment that examines the ef-
fects of nonfinancial performance feedback on the behavior of professionals
working for an insurance repair company. Wevary the frequency (weekly and
monthly) and the level of detail of the feedback that the 800 professionals
receive. Contrary to what we would expect if these professionals conformed
to the model of the Bayesian decision maker, more (and more frequent) in-
Arizona State University; ISEG, Universidade de Lisboa and Advance, CSG Research Cen-
ter; University of Notre Dame.
Accepted by Philip Berger. We thank Alberto Abadie, Larbi Alaoui, Ramji Balakrishnan,
Javier Bartolom´
e, Seth Berry,Margaret Christ, Juanjo Ganuza, Gar y Hecht, Chris Ittner,Michal
Matejka, Katherine Miller, Andrei Shleifer, Sriram Somanchi, two anonymous referees, and
seminar participants at the AAA Annual Meeting, Arizona State University-University of Ari-
zona Workshop, Carnegie Mellon University, IAP Cambridge Research Symposium, GMARS,
IE Business School, Midwest Summer Research Conference, MAS Midyear Meeting, North-
western University, Temple University Accounting Conference, Universidad Complutense de
Madrid, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, University of Notre Dame, and University of Toledo for
helpful comments. Sofia M. Lourenc¸o gratefully acknowledges financial support from FCT—
Fundac¸˜
ao para a Ciˆ
encia e Tecnologia (Portugal), research grant PTDC/EGE-GES/119607/2010
(national funding), and Pablo Casas-Arce from the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innova-
tion research grants MINECO ECO2011-28965 and MINECO ECO2014-59225-P.All errors are
our own.
1051
Copyright C,University of Chicago on behalf of the Accounting Research Center, 2017
1052 P.CASAS-ARCE,S.M.LOURENC¸O,AND F.A.MART´
INEZ-JEREZ
formation does not always help improve performance. In fact, we find that
professionals achieve the best outcomes when they receive detailed but infre-
quent (monthly) feedback. The treatment groups with frequent feedback, re-
gardless of how detailed it is, perform no better than the control group (with
monthly and aggregate information). The results are consistent with the in-
formation in the latest feedback report being most salient and professionals
in the weekly treatments overweighting their most recent performance, ham-
pering their ability to learn.
JEL codes: C93; D81; D91; J01; M40; M41; M52; M54; M55
Keywords: feedback; incentives; salience; performance; customer satisfac-
tion; field experiment
1. Introduction
One of the main roles of accounting information is to facilitate decision
making. Timeliness and detail are usually regarded as desirable character-
istics of information because they enable prompt and adequate responses
to business threats and opportunities. However, the intensity of these at-
tributes must be weighed against the decision maker’s ability to process the
relevant information. Too frequent information may result, for instance,
in an overreaction to short-term factors, whereas too detailed informa-
tion may cloud a decision maker’s ability to identify general trends or is-
sues. The progress of technology, dramatically expanding the amount of
data collected, and the speed with which this information can be made
available to managers for decision making, has exacerbated these ten-
sions. In this paper, we use a natural field experiment to analyze how
the frequency and detail of performance feedback influences employee
behavior.
In collaboration with Multiasistencia—the leading Spanish business pro-
cess outsourcer of repairs for insurance companies—we design and imple-
ment a field experiment in which we manipulate the nonfinancial per-
formance feedback received by 800 home repair professionals (such as
plumbers, masons, or painters) who work with the firm. We intervene in
the reward and feedback system by introducing a bonus that rewards the
achievement of a target for customer satisfaction—a metric that has been
found to be a significant leading indicator of the financial health of a firm
(Ittner and Larcker [1998])—as well as two process indicators. We vary the
frequency of feedback information (weekly vs. monthly) and the level of
detail included in the report (the average score of all jobs performed dur-
ing the period by a professional vs. the scores for each of the individual
jobs).
If the professionals in our setting conformed to the ideal of the Bayesian
decision maker, they would efficiently use all the information available to
them to improve customer satisfaction. Therefore, more detailed and more
THE PERFORMANCE EFFECT OF FEEDBACK FREQUENCY AND DETAIL 1053
frequent feedback should lead to better performance. However, more (and
more frequent) customer satisfaction feedback does not always result in
improved customer satisfaction scores. In fact, we find that professionals
achieve higher scores when they receive detailed but infrequent (monthly)
feedback. These results are consistent with the latest feedback report being
most salient, and professionals overweighting the information it contains.
As a result, although detailed customer satisfaction feedback supplies infor-
mation that helps professionals to improve the service they provide, feed-
back that is more frequent (and that consequently focuses on a shorter time
horizon) ends up harming service, as previous information is disregarded
in the face of new information.
Notably, we also show that the deterioration in performance for the pro-
fessionals in the weekly treatments cannot be explained by their rational
abandonment of the monthly bonus. It is true that professionals in weekly
treatments may review their decision to exert effort every time they receive
a feedback update. It is also true that, if they learn in a weekly update that
they are unlikely to qualify for the bonus, they may (rationally) decide to
abandon the pursuit of the monthly target, resulting in lower performance
than participants in monthly treatments, who do not receive weekly up-
dates. However, we find that such dynamic incentives cannot be the main
driver of our results, because weekly treatments do worse than monthly
treatments in the first week of the month, particularly when they follow a
negative report in the last week of the prior month. This result cannot be
explained by the rational abandonment story, as bad performance in the
last week of a month has no impact on the chances of achieving the bonus
in the next month.1
These differences among treatments do not exist with respect to the pro-
cess indicators included in the bonus system (e.g., the use of the Internet to
schedule a service or finishing a repair on time). This is because the profes-
sionals receive immediate feedback simply by executing these tasks. Thus,
the differences in the features of the formal feedback system do not result
in any additional information, and do not affect professionals’ knowledge
about their performance or the way they process that information (Annett
[1969]). Consistent with this, we show that operational performance is in-
deed the same in all four treatments.
This paper contributes to several streams of literature. First, we con-
tribute to the literature in information economics. In this literature, the
contracting stream of research on performance measurement mainly fo-
cuses on how properties of information affect their inclusion in contracts
(Feltham and Xie [1994], Prendergast [2002], Moers [2006]). The ulti-
mate objective of this literature is to judge the strength of the performance
metric in providing information to the firm about employees’ choices (the
1Moreover, there is no reason for professionals in the weekly treatments to be more likely
to infer that the bonus is not achievable. Both weekly and monthly treatments have the same
information about past performance during the first week of each month.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT