The Impact of Salience on Investor Behavior: Evidence from a Natural Experiment

DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/jofi.12851
AuthorCARY FRYDMAN,BAOLIAN WANG
Date01 February 2020
Published date01 February 2020
THE JOURNAL OF FINANCE VOL. LXXV, NO. 1 FEBRUARY 2020
The Impact of Salience on Investor Behavior:
Evidence from a Natural Experiment
CARY FRYDMAN and BAOLIAN WANG
ABSTRACT
Wetest whether the display of information causally affects investor behavior in a high-
stakes trading environment. Using investor-level brokerage data from China and a
natural experiment, we estimate the impact of a shock that increased the salience
of a stock’s purchase price but did not change the investor’s information set. We em-
ploy a difference-in-differences approach and find that the salience shock causally
increased the disposition effect by 17%. We use microdata to document substantial
heterogeneity across investors in the treatment effect. A previously documented trad-
ing pattern, the “rank effect,” explains heterogeneity in the change in the disposition
effect.
IN MOST MODELS OF INVESTOR behavior, the manner in which information is
displayed has little impact on portfolio choice. Yet, evidence from several
laboratory experiments indicates that information display can substantially
affect investor behavior. For example, manipulating the presentation of an
asset’s past performance can affect subjects’ risk-taking (Gneezy and Potters
(1997), Thaler et al. (1997)), mutual fund choice (Choi, Madrian, and Laibson
(2010)), and willingness to sell losing stocks (Frydman and Rangel (2014)).
Outside the laboratory, it is less clear whether a change in information
display would also have a large effect on trading decisions. One recent exper-
iment finds that when subjects make decisions over the course of a calendar
year—compared to a single laboratory session—the effect of information
display on portfolio risk-taking largely disappears (Beshears et al. (2017)).
This result highlights the importance of the experimental setting. Thus, there
Cary Frydman is at the Department of Finance and Business Economics, USC Marshall School
of Business. Baolian Wang is at Warrington College of Business, University of Florida. We are
thankful for comments from Kenneth Ahern; Nicholas Barberis; Itzhak Ben-David; Justin Birru;
Bing Han; Alex Imas; Danling Jiang; An Li; Juhani Linnainmaa; Mark Seasholes; and seminar
participants from Ohio State University, University of Southern California, Nankai University,
Renmin University in China, Peking University,University of Pennsylvania, the Cognition and De-
cision Laboratory at Columbia University,the 2016 Caltech Junior Behavioral Finance conference,
the 2017 Conference on China’s Financial Markets and Growth Rebalancing, the 2017 Interdisci-
plinary Symposium on Decision Neuroscience, the 2017 Miami Behavioral Finance Conference, the
2018 CFP Academic Research Colloquium for Financial Planning, and the 2018 China Financial
Research Conference. Frydman acknowledges financial support from NSF grant #1749824. The
authors have read The Journal of Finance’s disclosure policy and have no conflicts of interest to
disclose.
DOI: 10.1111/jofi.12851
C2019 the American Finance Association
229
230 The Journal of Finance R
is a need for additional empirical evidence from a higher-stakes and more
natural trading environment.
In this paper, we use a combination of microdata from a Chinese brokerage
house and a natural experiment to estimate the causal effect of a change in
information display on individual investor trading decisions. Specifically, we
obtain data from a brokerage house that increased the salience of a stock’s
capital gain by making it more visually prominent on the investor’s online
trading screen (Figure 1). To assess the impact on investor behavior, we
measure the disposition effect, which is the greater tendency to sell stocks
with capital gains compared to capital losses (Shefrin and Statman (1985),
Odean (1998)).1We hypothesize that increasing the salience of a stock’s capital
gain will increase the disposition effect.
In general, there are three main challenges in estimating the causal
effect of information display on investor behavior in the field. First, in many
circumstances, the display of information is correlated with the information
itself. For example, newspapers and online brokerages often prominently
display a list of daily winner and loser stocks, which confounds the content
of information (the daily return) with the salience of information (its display
on the list) (though see Kaniel and Parham (2017) and Wang (2019)). Here,
we use a unique shock to the display of information about a stock’s capital
gain that is uncorrelated with the capital gain itself. Because information on
the capital gain was available to investors before the change in information
display, this did not affect the information content of the capital gain, but
rather made it more salient and easier for the investor to process. We therefore
refer to the change as a “salience shock” rather than an information shock.
Second, estimating the effect on the average investor’s behavior can mask
important heterogeneity that makes the size of the effect hard to interpret.
In particular, investors who hold many stocks in their portfolio may exhibit
greater changes in behavior because more information becomes prominently
displayed on their trading screen. This could lead the average effect to be
overestimated if larger investors contribute more observations to the analysis
than smaller investors. Our data provide information on trades at the account
level, and thus we can estimate the effect at the individual level (Taubinsky
and Rees-Jones (2018)).
The third challenge is that the change in display may coincide with a
distinct but unobserved shock that also affects trading behavior. The salience
shock that we study generated a natural experiment that provides a control
group, which we use to control for common time-series variation in the
disposition effect. In particular, our data provide information on the method of
trade—internet, phone, or in-person (Barber and Odean (2002)). Because the
change in information display occurred online, the salience shock should not
1The disposition effect has been documented among both individual and professional investors.
It is an extremely robust effect that has been found among a wide variety of asset classes and
international markets. For a comprehensive review of the disposition effect, see Kaustia (2010)
and Barber and Odean (2013).
Impact of Salience on Investor Behavior 231
Panel A: Before October 2004
Panel B: After October 2004
3073.44 -11.02 -0.36% 1270 10694.5 -89.84 -0.83%1934 6908.67 -64.56 -0.93%817.3 3312.84 -5.76 -0.17%701.4 2171.64 -21.62 -0.99%557.7
3073.44 -11.02 -0.36% 1270 10694.5 -89.84 -0.83%1934 6908.67 -64.56 -0.93% 817.3 3312.84 -5.76 -0.17%701.4 2171.64 -21.62 -0.99% 557.7
StockID Name Shares SharesS BrkEvn PrchPrice Price MktValue PaperGain RlzdGain TotalGain FrznShr TraderID Non-tradable
StockID Name Shares SharesS Price MktValue FrznShrs TraderID Non-tradable
144933.30 144933.30 142857.00 287790.30
600769
000002
002728
6000
2300
600
6000
2300
600
11.390
25.290
27.250
68340.000
58167.000
16350.000
0
0
0
0
0
0
A250597397
0137929051
0137929061
600769
002728
6000
600 600
6000 11.884
30.248
12.672
30.209
11.39 0
27.250
25.290
68340. 000
16350. 000
-2966. 770
-1799. 010 0.000
-4820. 530 -7787. 300
-1799. 010
0
0
0
0
A250597397
0137929051
000002 2300 2300 24.140 24.109 58167. 000 2644. 210 0. 000 2644. 210 00137929051 0
144933.30 144933.30 142857.00 287790.30
Figure 1. Change in information display. Panel A shows an example of the trading platform screen prior to the salience shock in October 2004.
Panel B shows an example of the trading platform screen after the salience shock, when five variables were added: (I) break-even price (column
#5), (II) weighted-average purchase price (column #6), (III) paper gain/loss (column #9), (IV) realized gain/loss (column #10), and (V) total gain/loss
(column #11). After the salience shock, the font color for each stock was a function of whether the stock was trading at a gain (red) or at a loss (blue).
The English translations in both panels and the red rectangles highlighting the added variables in the bottom panel are for display purposes only
and were not presented to investors.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT