Student Perceptions of a New Campus Alcohol Policy: Linking Deterrence and Blame Attribution

Date01 July 2017
DOI10.1177/0022042617699195
Published date01 July 2017
AuthorMargaret S. Kelley
Subject MatterArticles
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022042617699195
Journal of Drug Issues
2017, Vol. 47(3) 411 –432
© The Author(s) 2017
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0022042617699195
journals.sagepub.com/home/jod
Article
Student Perceptions of a New
Campus Alcohol Policy: Linking
Deterrence and Blame Attribution
Margaret S. Kelley1
Abstract
How do students make judgments about their future behaviors involving the use of alcohol? The
present study advances deterrence theory by introducing elements of attribution theory while
examining the perceived deterrent effects of a newly instituted dry policy on a college campus.
A phone survey of 508 full-time undergraduate students between the ages of 18 and 26 was
conducted 1 year following the ban on alcohol. Hypotheses are presented in two models and
predict that deterrence (surveillance and enforcement) and blame explain intentions to violate
the alcohol policy. These relationships are expected to be mediated by problem attribution and
shame and are moderated by gender. Results show that perceived peer surveillance does reduce
intentions to violate the policy in the future but formal enforcement does not. Findings further
indicate that attributing blame acts to increase the likelihood of experiencing shame, therefore,
reducing intentions to violate the alcohol policy. Gender conditioned the effects of surveillance
and shame (stronger for females) on future policy violations. Problems that arise from drinking
can be understood as another form of attribution that acts to mediate surveillance. Surveillance
and blame contribute to the judgment process as students think about violating the alcohol
policy, and shame emerges as the key link between deterrence and attribution.
Keywords
blame attribution, shame, surveillance, deterrence, alcohol policy, gender
Introduction
The primary objective of this article is to contribute to ongoing efforts to understand and regulate
problem drinking in college through the examination of the perceived deterrence effects of a
recently enacted dry policy at a large southwestern state university. Existing research on the
impact of this policy has used deterrence theory as the framework for examining the formal and
informal sanctions embedded in it, concluding that the informal sanction of shame is more effec-
tive than the potential formal consequences of getting caught (Kelley, Miyuki, Spivak, & Payne,
2009). In addition, it was revealed that religiosity increased perceived threats of shame and
embarrassment, which in turn reduced the likelihood of anticipated policy violations (Spivak,
Fukushima, Kelley, & Sanford-Jenson, 2011).
1University of Kansas, Lawrence, USA
Corresponding Author:
Margaret S. Kelley, University of Kansas, Department of American Studies, 1440 Jayhawk Blvd., Lawrence, KS 66045,
USA.
Email: mskelley@ku.edu
699195JODXXX10.1177/0022042617699195Journal of Drug IssuesKelley
research-article2017
412 Journal of Drug Issues 47(3)
In the current analysis, I extend my examination of the campus alcohol policy by analyzing a
second wave of trend data collected a year after the policy was put into place (N = 508). I exam-
ine the linkage between formal and informal sources of social control in the tradition of studies
in perceptual deterrence (Anderson, Harris, & Miller, 1983; Nagin, 1998) and introduce two new
categories of variables. Perceptual deterrence allows us to evaluate intentions to comply with or
violate rules and regulations, also known as projected offending, and deterrence theory has been
tested by examining perceptions of punishment and law violations (Klepper & Nagin, 1989;
Lanza-Kaduce, 1988; Liska & Messner, 1999; Nagin & Pogarsky, 2003; Pogarsky, 2004). Future
intentions are recognized as a legitimate way of operationalizing some types of behavior, although
predictions by respondents are not assumed to be completely accurate (Alreck & Settle, 2004).
First, derived from deterrence theory, I use perceived surveillance, enforcement, and shame to
predict intentions to violate. Second, from attribution theory, I incorporate measures of blame
attribution. These together paint a unique picture of the ways that deterrence theory works in the
decision making of young people about their drinking behaviors.
Problem Drinking
Problems with alcohol on college campuses are certainly not new. Indeed, drinking is often con-
sidered a rite of passage into college life and plays a central role in the college experience
(Inciardi, 2002; Kuo, Wechsler, Greenberg, & Lee, 2003; Straus & Shelden, 1953; Workman,
2001). Because of widespread American acceptance of drinking as a part of college life, college
students tend to show a strong resistance toward policies regulating alcohol use on campus
(Harris, Sherritt, Van Hook, Wechsler, & Knight, 2010; Wechsler, Lee, Gledhill-Hoyt, & Nelson,
2001), even when confronted with problems associated with drinking and “living the party life.”
Binge drinking, or heavy episodic drinking, has commonly been defined by researchers as the
consumption of five or more drinks on a single occasion for males and four or more for females
(a more technical definition takes into account blood alcohol concentration levels; Inciardi, 2002;
Lange et al., 2002; National Institutes of Health, 2007; Wechsler, Lee, Kuo, & Lee, 2000). Binge
drinking as a public health threat on campus has captured increasing attention in recent years
(Chaloupka & Wechsler, 1996; Wechsler, Dowdall, Davenport, & DeJong, 1993; Wechsler,
Dowdall, Maenner, Gledhill-Hoyt, & Lee, 1998; Williams, Chaloupka, & Wechsler, 2005).
Today, virtually every university in the United States provides some kind of alcohol intervention
program for students. One way to categorize popular efforts to control consumption is as using
dry or wet policies. A dry policy is developed with punishments designed to curb drinking behav-
iors, similar to Prohibition. Other schools implement a wet policy, which provides a controlled
environment for students to learn to drink responsibly (Workman, 2001).
There may be unintended consequences of broad bans against alcohol use on campus. For
example, some research shows that rather than growing the number of careful and responsible
drinkers, bans had a polarizing effect (Wechsler, Lee, Kuo, & Seibring, 2002). Yes, more students
abstained, but more moderate drinkers crossed over into excessive and problem drinking. The
literature is clear that the more students drink, the more likely they are to experience problems in
their lives directly related to their use of alcohol. Schools are very interested in minimizing harm
to students by decreasing the negative impact of excessive drinking.
Informal Deterrence and Alcohol Policies
We know that informal sanctions, such as the negative consequences deriving from reactions of
others close to the individual, appear to have a stronger deterrent effect on individuals than for-
mal sanctions (Foglia, 1997; Tittle & Paternoster, 2000). Research in deterrence theory has con-
cluded that “the extralegal consequences of crime seem at least as great a deterrent as do the legal

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT