Revisiting the trustworthiness–trust relationship: Exploring the differential predictors of cognition‐ and affect‐based trust

Date01 July 2020
Published date01 July 2020
AuthorAndrew K. Schnackenberg,Edward C. Tomlinson,Steven R. Ash,David Dawley
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1002/job.2448
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Revisiting the trustworthinesstrust relationship: Exploring the
differential predictors of cognition- and affect-based trust
Edward C. Tomlinson
1
| Andrew K. Schnackenberg
2
| David Dawley
1
|
Steven R. Ash
3
1
Department of Management, West Virginia
University, Morgantown, West Virginia, USA
2
Management Department, University of
Denver, Denver, Colorado, USA
3
Department of Management, University of
Akron, Akron, Ohio, USA
Correspondence
Edward C. Tomlinson, West Virginia
University, Morgantown, WV, USA.
Email: edward.tomlinson@mail.wvu.edu
Funding information
WVU College of Business & Economics
research funding
Summary
We seek to develop a better understanding of interpersonal trust by bridging the gap
between two heretofore distinct paradigms of trust. One paradigm views trust in
terms of two dimensions: cognition- and affect-based. The other paradigm views
trust as being distinct from trustworthiness, which has four dimensions: ability,
behavioral integrity, benevolence, and values congruence. Currently, theoretical con-
sensus is lacking about the antecedents of cognition- and affect-based trust in the
first paradigm that incorporates insights from research on trustworthiness in the sec-
ond paradigm. We show that this lack of consensus is problematic for internal knowl-
edge development and external knowledge expansion. Thus, we join both paradigms
by theorizing that ability and behavioral integrity are the most important predictors
of cognition-based trust, whereas benevolence and values congruence are the most
important predictors of affect-based trust. Across two samples, we found that our
predictions were largely supported. Based on relative weights analysis, ability and
behavioral integrity were more important than values congruence in predicting
cognition-based trust, and benevolence was more important than ability in predicting
affect-based trust. Furthermore, we found evidence that these relationships were
largely robust to changes in the referent of analysis.
KEYWORDS
affect, cognition, relative weights analysis, trust, trustworthiness
1|INTRODUCTION
Research has yielded a variety of insights on the importance of culti-
vating trusting interpersonal relationships in the workplace. Among
these findings, there is robust evidence of a positive relationship
between trust and various work-related attitudes and performance
behaviors (e.g., Colquitt, Scott, & LePine, 2007; Dirks & Ferrin, 2002).
For example, trust in one's supervisor is associated with greater self-
efficacy, psychological safety, job performance (Li & Tan, 2013),
knowledge sharing (Nerstad et al., 2018), and job engagement
(Haynie, Mossholder, & Harris, 2016).
Accordingly, management researchers have devoted considerable
effort to understanding interpersonal trust. Since the mid-1990s, a
substantial body of trust research has fallen into one of two distinct
but nonexclusive paradigms. The first paradigm, which we label as
cognition- and affect-based trust research (C/ABT), distinguishes
between two major dimensions of trust. Trust in another can be char-
acterized as cognition-based trust (CBT; i.e., based on rational calcula-
tion) and affect-based trust (ABT; i.e., based on emotional
attachment). Within this paradigm, researchers have established that
separate dimensions of trust are useful to organizations for different
purposes. For example, CBT mediates the relationship between trans-
formational leadership and task performance, whereas ABT mediates
the relationship between transformational leadership and job satisfac-
tion (Y. Zhu & Akhtar, 2014b). Prior studies have also uncovered a
number of antecedents of each trust dimension. For instance, Dunn,
Received: 1 October 2018 Revised: 27 March 2020 Accepted: 20 April 2020
DOI: 10.1002/job.2448
J Organ Behav. 2020;41:535550. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/job © 2020 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 535
Ruedy, and Schweitzer (2012) found that upward social comparisons
in organizations (i.e., comparisons with someone whose performance
is superior to one's own) could harm ABT, whereas downward social
comparisons (i.e., comparisons with someone whose performance is
inferior to one's own) could harm CBT.
The second paradigm, trustworthiness and trust (TW/T) research,
distinguishes between the factors that give rise to interpersonal trust
(i.e., trustworthiness factors) and trust itself. Within this paradigm,
researchers have examined how trustworthiness factors underlie the
cultivation of trust (as a unidimensional construct) across a variety of
organizational contexts and conditions. For example, Mayer and
Gavin (2005) showed that trust in one's manager was based on the
manager's perceived ability, benevolence, and integrity. In the context
of organizational recruitment and selection, Klotz, da Motta Veiga,
Buckley, and Gavin (2013) posited that the trustworthiness of organi-
zational agents (e.g., hiring managers) is vital to facilitate job applicant
trust in the hiring organization. Frazier, Tupper, and Fainshmidt (2016)
found that the route to high trust in one's supervisor differed between
early stage and established relationships: In early stage relationships,
high supervisor ability, benevolence, and integrity were sufficient; but
in established relationships, either high supervisor ability and integrity
or high supervisor ability and benevolence, in conjunction with high
propensity to trust, were sufficient.
Despite substantial research within each paradigm, consensus
knowledge is currently lacking about the antecedents of C/ABT in the
first tradition that integrates insights from research on trustworthi-
ness in the second tradition. This is problematic for two reasons. First,
consensus about conceptual interrelationships within any domain of
study is necessary to enhance researchers' abilities to pursue key
questions that advance core knowledge (Hollenbeck, 2008). For
example, researchers have examined contextual variables (e.g., social
comparisons, reputation, source expertise, leadership styles, etc.;
Dirks & Ferrin, 2002; Dunn et al., 2012; Johnson & Grayson, 2005)
for their impact on C/ABT without offering a robust theoretical
grounding of their causal sequencing or the logic guiding their selec-
tion of specific antecedents. As the proximal antecedents of trust,
trustworthiness factors are the most important predictors of trust
(Corbitt, Thanasankit, & Yi, 2003, p. 210) such that contextual ante-
cedents (e.g., reputation or social comparisons) should only predict
trust to the extent that they influence the trustor's perception of the
trustee's trustworthiness. Despite this generally accepted principle in
the trust literature, we know surprisingly little about the conditions of
trustworthiness that give rise to each dimension of trust. Thus, exis-
ting knowledge about the antecedents of trust types is unsystematic
largely due to lack of internal consensus in the trust literature about
the interrelationships among trustworthiness dimensions (as the prox-
imal antecedents of trust) and trust types.
Second, given trust is perhaps one of the most important and
widely applied concepts across domains of research in the organiza-
tional literature, consensus about the interrelationships between
trustworthiness dimensions and trust types have potentially far-
reaching consequences. For example, in psychological contracts
research, C/ABT have been found to underpin transactional and
relational psychological contracts, respectively (Atkinson, 2007). How-
ever, the specific forms of trustworthiness that develop CBT versus
ABT are unknown. In leadership research, transformational leaders
have been shown to build more CBT than ABT, but transformational
leaders who engender ABT are generally more effective
(N. A. Gillespie & Mann, 2004; W. Zhu, Newman, Miao, &
Hooke, 2013; Y. Zhu & Akhtar, 2014a). However, the trustworthiness
dimensions needed for transformational leaders to cultivate more
affect-based (i.e., performance enhancing) trust are unknown.
Although these and other studies have generated a variety of useful
insights, open questions remain that are only possible to advance in a
systematic fashion with greater consensus knowledge about the theo-
retical interrelationships among trustworthiness dimensions and trust
types.
Thus, in this study, we pursue consensus knowledge about the
antecedents of trust types by examining the interrelationships among
trustworthiness dimensions and C/ABT specifically, with implications
for advancing the understanding of related domains where interper-
sonal trust is commonly studied. Before we proceed, we point out
that our efforts here represent an important initial step in consensus
generation, but we in no way wish to preclude subsequent scholarly
debate. Indeed, consensus exists around many ideas in the organiza-
tion sciences that continue to be richly debated as the meaning and
boundary conditions of those ideas are stretched and incorporated
into other scholarly domains. Our pursuit of consensus intends to
highlight the importance of understanding the basic properties and
interrelationships among focal theoretical constructs in trust research.
2|THEORETICAL BACKGROUND
To develop our framework, we begin by considering extant theory
within each paradigm in isolation and then proceed to integrate the
two paradigms of trust research. Specifically, we draw from theory
and empirical research in each paradigm to develop hypotheses of
how these distinct paradigms relate to each other (cf. Caza, Vough, &
Puranik, 2018; Martinko & Thomson, 1998).
2.1 |The dimensions of trust (C/ABT)
McAllister (1995) argues that trust refers to the extent to which a
person is confident in, and willing to act on the basis of, the words,
actions, and decisions of another(p. 25). Building on earlier research
(Johnson-George & Swap, 1982; Lewis & Weigert, 1985; Rempel,
Holmes, & Zanna, 1985), McAllister (1995) proposed two fundamen-
tally different dimensions of trust: cognition-based trust, grounded in
individual beliefs about peer reliability and dependability, and affect-
based trust, grounded in reciprocated interpersonal care and concern
(p. 25). Chua, Ingram, and Morris (2008) refer to these dimensions as
trust from the headand trust from the heart,respectively.
McAllister's model has become so widely used that it is now a stan-
dard part of the vernacular in trust research.
536 TOMLINSON ET AL.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT