Retaliatory Auto Theft

AuthorBruce A. Jacobs,Michael Cherbonneau
Date01 November 2015
DOI10.1177/1043986215608533
Published date01 November 2015
Subject MatterArticles
Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice
2015, Vol. 31(4) 468 –491
© 2015 SAGE Publications
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1043986215608533
ccj.sagepub.com
Article
Retaliatory Auto Theft
Michael Cherbonneau1 and Bruce A. Jacobs2
Abstract
Drawing from a qualitative sample of active auto thieves, this article examines the
moralistic underpinnings of auto theft. Our findings indicate that retaliatory auto theft
is either direct or indirect. In direct forms of payback, auto theft reprises a specific
violator for a specific affront, and the theft serves as that reprisal. In indirect payback,
the target’s culpability is lacking: Auto theft either removes some generalized loss or
facilitates a broader retributive objective secondary to the theft target. Discussion
focuses on the distinction between revenge and retribution and how auto theft
emerges as a feasible choice given the universe of available retaliatory options, and
despite the longstanding preference among street offenders for violence.
Keywords
retaliation, offender decision-making, motor vehicle theft
Introduction
While much is understood about decision-making in predatory street crimes such as
robbery and burglary (e.g., Bennett & Wright, 1984; Cromwell & Olson, 2004; Gabor
et al., 1987; Hochstetler, 2001; Jacobs, 2000; Jacobs, Topalli, & Wright, 2003; Katz,
1991; Shover, 1996; Topalli & Wright, 2004; Tunnell, 1992; Walsh, 1986; R. T. Wright
& Decker, 1994, 1997; R. Wright, Brookman, & Bennett, 2006), relatively little is
known about the core decision-making processes of auto theft (but see, Cherbonneau
& Copes, 2006, 2012; Fleming, 1999; Jacobs & Cherbonneau, 2014a, 2014b; Light,
Nee, & Ingham, 1993). Even less is known about the etiology of this crime exclusive
of financial or expressive motives (but see, Copes, 2003, pp. 324-326). Drawing from
a qualitative sample of active auto thieves, we address that lacuna here by examining
1University of North Florida, Jacksonville, FL, USA
2University of Texas at Dallas, Richardson, TX, USA
Corresponding Author:
Michael Cherbonneau, Criminology and Criminal Justice Department, University of North Florida, 1
UNF Drive, Jacksonville, FL 32224, USA.
Email: m.cherbonneau@unf.edu
608533CCJXXX10.1177/1043986215608533Journal of Contemporary Criminal JusticeCherbonneau and Jacobs
research-article2015
Cherbonneau and Jacobs 469
the moralistic basis of auto theft and its conceptual implications for retributive
decision-making.
Offender Decision-Making and Street Criminal
Retaliation
In line with the recent interest in criminal decision-making, researchers have endeav-
ored to explore the risks and rewards that offenders attach to specific crimes and how
those perceptions guide criminal choice (e.g., Bennett & Wright, 1984; Carroll &
Weaver, 1986; Copes & Cherbonneau, 2014; Cromwell & Olson, 2004; Feeney, 1986;
Hochstetler & Copes, 2003; Jacobs, 1999; Shover, 1996; Tunnell, 1992; R. T. Wright
& Decker, 1994, 1997). Persistent street offenders have been the subject of an appre-
ciable share of this research (see, for example, Brookman, Bennett, Hochstetler, &
Copes, 2011; Copes, 2003; Hochstetler, 2002; Jacobs, 2000; Katz, 1991; Rosenfeld,
Jacobs, & Wright, 2003; Shover & Honaker, 1992). This state of affairs is not surpris-
ing: Street offenders commit a disproportionate amount of serious violent and property
crime and consume significant criminal justice resources in attempts to interdict and
deter them (Sherman, 2007; Wolfgang, Figlio, & Sellin, 1972).
Generally speaking, the study of offender decision-making is nested within several
broader theoretical traditions, which include deterrence, rational choice, and routine
activities (Clarke & Cornish, 1985; Clarke & Felson, 1993; Cornish & Clarke, 1986;
Felson & Boba, 2009; Gibbs, 1975; Jacobs, 2010; Nagin, 1998; Paternoster, 2010;
Piquero & Tibbetts, 2002). Each of these perspectives is unique, but all tend to under-
emphasize the role of situational context in offending decisions (Jacobs & Wright,
1999; Von Hirsch, Bottoms, Burney, & Wilkström, 1999). Indeed, a hallmark of stud-
ies in this tradition is the assumption of “attenuated positivism” (Lofland, 1969),
wherein offender agency is bypassed or presumed outright in favor of softly determin-
istic views of criminal conduct (see Clarke & Cornish, 1985).
This tendency owes largely to quantitative, survey-based research designs that dom-
inate inquiry in this area (see Loughran, Pogarsky, Piquero, & Paternoster, 2011;
Matsueda, Kreager, & Huizinga, 2006; Paternoster, 1989; Peterson & Braiker, 1980;
Piquero & Rengert, 1999). Because survey methodology explores decision-making in
the abstract, it limits the scope of research to a small range of decisions that link con-
temporaneous risk/reward perceptions to hypothetical crime participation and projected
offending (Apel, 2013; Von Hirsch et al., 1999). While useful for testing core tenets of
decision-making theory, this approach tends to truncate criminal choice and presuppose
its nuanced nature (Topalli, 2011). This is unfortunate given that all social action—
including crime—is nested in, and shaped by, context (R. T. Wright & Decker, 1994).
Renowned for their ability to overcome these limitations are qualitative, ethno-
graphic field-based studies of offenders (Topalli, 2011). Here, sample size and gener-
alizability—the bedrock of quantitative research—are sacrificed in favor of relatively
small, targeted samples of known offenders to address the nuances of criminal deci-
sion-making (see Bennett & Wright, 1984; Carroll & Weaver, 1986; Copes & Vieraitis,
2012; Cromwell & Olson, 2004; Jacobs, 1999, 2000; Jacobs & Cherbonneau, 2014a;

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT