On being nice and effective: Work‐related emotional intelligence and its role in conflict resolution and interpersonal problem‐solving

AuthorMichael D. Robinson,Buddhika Perera,Kay Hopkins,Sukumarakurup Krishnakumar
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1002/crq.21268
Published date01 December 2019
Date01 December 2019
RESEARCH ARTICLE
On being nice and effective: Work-related emotional
intelligence and its role in conflict resolution and
interpersonal problem-solving
Sukumarakurup Krishnakumar
1
| Buddhika Perera
2
|
Kay Hopkins
3
| Michael D. Robinson
3
1
Keck Graduate Institute (Claremont
Colleges), Claremont, California
2
The Asia Foundation, Colombo, Sri Lanka
3
North Dakota State University, Fargo,
North Dakota
Correspondence
Sukumarakurup Krishnakumar, Keck
Graduate Institute (Claremont Colleges),
Keck Graduate Institute, 535 Watson Drive,
Claremont, CA 91711.
Email: kurup.mgmt@gmail.com
Abstract
People are thought to differ in their abilities to perceive,
understand, and manage emotions, a construct termed emo-
tional intelligence (EI). North Dakota emotional abilities test
(NEAT), a test of EI based on the situation judgment test
method, assesses EI applied to work settings. Three survey-
based studies examined and found that NEAT scores corre-
lated positively with constructive motivations and behavioral
intentions (Study 1; n= 94), conflict handling strategies
favoring mutual interests (Study 2; n= 92), and helpful
workplace behaviors, particularly under stressful conditions
(Study 3; n= 90). These findings could possess particular
value in domains such as conflict resolution and mediation.
1|INTRODUCTION
Emotions can sometimes occur in the absence of social stimulation, such as when encountering a
snake in the woods. However, the vast majority of our emotions seem to occur in social situations
(Hareli & Parkinson, 2008). Our fears are typically social fears, our joys are typically social joys,
and many emotionssuch as anger, guilt, embarrassment, and loveeither require a social object or
an appraisal of the self as a social object (Hareli & Parkinson, 2008). In fact, interacting with others
often means subjecting oneself to a nearly constant stream of feelings, some of which can be subtle
or change fairly quickly (Russell, 2003). Meanwhile, our interaction partners, too, have emotional
states and these need to be considered if we want to be interpersonally successful, whether within or
outside the workplace (Jordan & Troth, 2002).
Given this overlap of social and emotional processes, individuals who are more attuned to the
emotions of self and others may tend to interact with others in more effective ways (Halberstadt,
Received: 3 June 2019 Revised: 8 August 2019 Accepted: 26 August 2019
DOI: 10.1002/crq.21268
Conflict Resolution Quarterly. 2019;37:147167. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/crq © 2019 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 147
Denham, & Dunsmore, 2001). Ideas of this type are prominent in multiple literatures. For example,
children who make incorrect inferences about the emotions of others seem prone to socially problem-
atic behaviors like aggression or withdrawal (Crick & Dodge, 1994). Adults who lack clarity con-
cerning their emotions also engage in problematic social behaviors, and processes of this type have
been implicated in a number of disorders (Gratz & Tull, 2010). Conversely, people who are intelli-
gent about emotions should be more capable of generating behaviors that are appropriate to the social
context, in part because they are able to down-regulate emotional reactions that are inappropriate
(Halberstadt et al., 2001).
In concert with these ideas, Salovey and Mayer (1990) proposed a dimension of emotional intelli-
gence (EI) that distinguishes people who are insightful concerning emotions from those who are not.
Emotionally intelligent individuals are thought to be more attuned to the emotions of self and others
while possessing a more sophisticated, nuanced understanding of the manner in which emotions
work (Mayer & Salovey, 1997). Moreover, emotionally intelligent individuals should be better able
to manage their emotions, which will often result in behaviors that are more skilled and tactful
(MacCann & Roberts, 2008). Individuals who lack EI, by contrast, would be slow to recognize emo-
tional processes, would not understand them well, and would be prone to emotional outbursts that
could exacerbate interpersonal problems rather than resolving them (Halberstadt et al., 2001; Lopes
et al., 2011).
Despite this relatively solid theoretical foundation, the EI field has been marked by a considerable
degree of controversy and confusion (Zeidner, Roberts, & Matthews, 2008). A major reason for this
state of affairs is that researchers have created self-report measures of what is essentially an ability-
related construct. This has resulted in a somewhat confusing mix of findings as well as questions
about whether EI simply remolds existing personality traits into new forms (Zeidner et al., 2008).
Fortunately, these problems can be solved by reserving the term EI for tests that adopt ability-related
procedures (Ashkanasy & Daus, 2005). Unfortunately, there are not a lot of tests of this type, some
must be purchased if one wants to use them, and there are limitations to the tests that currently exist
(Roberts, MacCann, Matthews, & Zeidner, 2010). For example, the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso EI Test
(the MSCEIT) is copyrighted in a way that limits its use, and there are also questions about this test's
reliability and validity (Maul, 2012; Palmer, Gignac, Manocha, & Stough, 2005).
Such considerations led to the development of a new ability EI measure with some promising
attributes. Rather than using tasks with unknown social significance (e.g., judging the emotions con-
veyed by abstract paintings), Krishnakumar, Hopkins, Szmerekovsky, and Robinson (2016) embed-
ded all of their North Dakota emotional abilities test (NEAT) items within a situational judgment test
(SJT) format. That is, the NEAT assesses perception, understanding, and management abilities in
relation to human characters encountering largely interpersonal (Lievens & Chan, 2010) challenges
and opportunities. Furthermore, consistent with the SJT literature (Whetzel & McDaniel, 2009), the
NEAT's scenarios focus on workplace events and outcomes rather than those that might occur out-
side of the workplace (e.g., conversations with one's neighbor). The contextual nature of this framing
should be advantageous in predicting workplace functioning (Shaffer & Postlethwaite, 2012). In
addition, though, workplace situations are useful, because they encourage test-takers to consider both
pragmatic/agentic and communal sources of concern when responding.
The NEAT correlates with other ability EI tests such as the STEU and the STEM (MacCann &
Roberts, 2008) and seems to predict workplace outcomes fairly well. Employees with higher NEAT
scores report lesser job stress, greater job satisfaction, and better job performance (Krishnakumar
et al., 2016). Also, interpersonal processes have been implicated in some of these findings. For exam-
ple, individuals with higher levels of work-related EI (W-EI) report being particularly satisfied with
148 KRISHNAKUMAR ET AL.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT