Not all Risk Taking is Born Equal: The Behavioral Agency Model and CEO's Perception of Firm Efficacy

AuthorMarianna Makri,Luis R. Gomez‐Mejia,Geoffrey Martin,Nathan Washburn
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1002/hrm.21624
Published date01 May 2015
Date01 May 2015
Human Resource Management, May–June 2015, Vol. 54, No. 3. Pp. 483–498
© 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Published online in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com).
DOI:10.1002/hrm.21624
Correspondence to: Luis R. Gomez-Mejia, Mendoza College of Business, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, IN
46556-5646, Phone: 574-631-3809, E-mail: lgomezme@nd.edu
NOT ALL RISK TAKING IS BORN
EQUAL: THE BEHAVIORAL AGENCY
MODEL AND CEO’S PERCEPTION
OFFIRM EFFICACY
GEOFFREY MARTIN, NATHAN WASHBURN,
MARIANNA MAKRI, AND LUIS R. GOMEZ-MEJIA
We examine the relationship between agent (CEO) risk bearing and the quality
of executive risk-taking outcomes, by examining the contingency effect of CEO
perceived fi rm effi cacy. In doing so, we extend the behavioral agency model
(BAM) beyond predictions of risk magnitude to examining how CEO risk-taking
outcomes differ qualitatively in response to risk bearing. We argue that CEO risk
bearing (due to stock options or cash compensation) will positively infl uence per-
formance outcomes in the presence of higher perceived fi rm effi cacy. However,
this positive infl uence reverses when effi cacy is lower. We demonstrate the utility
of fi rm effi cacy in exploring the effect of agent risk bearing on performance out-
comes and provide the insight that the CEO pay-performance relationship is
infl uenced by the CEO’s perception of fi rm ef cacy. © 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Keywords: behavioral agency, perceived fi rm effi cacy, risk, R&D
In this study, we argue that the relationship
between agent risk bearing and firm perfor-
mance is dependent on the CEO’s perception
of firm efficacy. We do so by drawing upon
the behavioral agency model (BAM; Wiseman
& Gomez-Mejia, 1998) and literature developing
the concept of firm efficacy. The BAM as origi-
nally formulated draws upon agent risk bearing,
such as risk bearing associated with executive
compensation, to predict the magnitude of agent
risk taking. For instance, numerous studies have
utilized the BAM to predict a negative relation-
ship between agent (CEO) risk bearing and risk
taking (Devers, McNamara, Wiseman, & Arrfelt,
2008; Larraza-Kintana, Wiseman, Gomez-Mejia,
& Welbourne, 2007; Martin, Gomez-Mejia, &
Wiseman, 2013). Advancing this line of research,
our study extends the BAM in two significant
ways. First, rather than predict risk magnitude, we
utilize the BAM to examine performance outcomes
(success of risk taking) associated with agent risk
bearing. Second, we incorporate the CEO’s percep-
tion of firm efficacy as a contingency influencing
the performance effect of agent risk bearing. In
doing so, we address a question that has received
little research attention within the agency litera-
ture: When or under what conditions is agent risk
bearing good or bad for shareholders?
Agency research has previously suggested that
executive risk bearing generated through com-
pensation—and the risk aversion it leads to—
will negatively influence performance outcomes
(Coffee, 1998; Jensen & Meckling, 1976; Wiseman
& Gomez-Mejia, 1998). The logic behind this
484 HUMAN RESOURCE MANAGEMENT, MAY–JUNE 2015
Human Resource Management DOI: 10.1002/hrm
The failure to
pursue higher-risk
but higher-value
projects is argued to
create agency costs
for shareholders
due to suboptimal
investment choices
and associated
inferior performance
outcomes.
and outcomes (Lindsley et al., 1995), this virtu-
ous cycle of success-induced efficacy generating
future successes has yet to be applied to the study
of executive risk taking and the performance con-
sequences of agent risk bearing. Given the CEO’s
perception of firm efficacy appears to influence
their search for and choice of strategic alterna-
tives, we argue that it is likely to influence the
quality of risk taking and therefore the perfor-
mance outcomes associated with that risk taking.
We do so by examining the moderation effect of
CEO perception of firm efficacy on the relation-
ship between CEO risk bearing and performance
outcomes.
Drawing on a sample of 297 US publicly
traded technology-intensive firms, we find sup-
port for our prediction that the CEO’s perception
of firm efficacy positively moderates the relation-
ship between CEO risk bearing and R&D perfor-
mance outcomes. That is, when the CEO perceives
higher levels of firm efficacy, CEO risk bearing is
positively related to R&D performance outcomes.
We capture R&D performance using invention
resonance—the extent to which patents registered
(in the previous five years) by the firm are cited by
subsequent patents.
Our findings make important contributions
to agency and executive compensation literature.
First, we demonstrate the utility of combining the
concept of efficacy with BAM to predict the out-
comes associated with CEO risk bearing. Second,
we provide the insight that CEO risk bearing can
be good for shareholders when the CEO perceives
higher firm efficacy. This suggests that tradi-
tional agency theory’s preoccupation with limit-
ing risk bearing in order to encourage agent risk
taking may not necessarily be in the interests of
the firm’s shareholders. Third, research examin-
ing the relationship between executive pay and
firm performance has generally failed to provide
support for the relationship between equity-based
pay and firm performance that agency theory
predicts (for a review of this literature, see Dalton
etal., 2003). Our study suggests that in order to
understand how CEO compensation influences
performance outcomes, it is necessary to consider
(1) the risk bearing created by CEO compensation
and (2) the CEO’s perception of firm efficacy. This
is useful for both agency scholars and practitio-
ners such as boards of directors and compensation
committees who are battling with the dilemma of
how to compensate their executives to encourage
sound risk taking. This is an issue that has taken
on greater importance in the wake of the global
financial crisis (see Basel Committee on Banking
Supervision, 2009). Finally, we contribute to stud-
ies of the performance effects of R&D and risk
argument is that risk-averse executives may opt
out of risky, potentially value-adding strategic
projects. They are suggested to do so because
their concentrated human and financial capital
(the latter due to enforced equity holdings) cre-
ates significant risk bearing (or wealth at risk of
loss) that reduces their appetite for risk taking.
According to this logic, managerial agents (such as
CEOs) choose less valuable but more conservative
strategic options (Gomez-Mejia, Berrone, Franco-
Santos, 2010). The failure to pursue higher-risk
but higher-value projects is argued to create
agency costs for shareholders due to suboptimal
investment choices and associated inferior per-
formance outcomes (Jensen & Meckling, 1976).
Empirical research, however, has failed to pro-
vide strong support for the relationship between
compensation-induced risk aversion
and poor performance outcomes
(Dalton, Daily, Certo, Roengpitya,
2003). In order to advance this line
of research and develop a stronger
understanding of the circumstances
under which agent risk bearing
could positively or negatively influ-
ence research and development
(R&D) performance, we examine
CEO decision making with regard
to their R&D investment strategy
and the outcomes of these decisions
contingent on the CEO’s perception
of firm efficacy.
Efficacy is defined as “the con-
viction that one can successfully
execute the behavior required”
(Bandura, 1977, p. 193). The rela-
tionship between efficacy and per-
formance has been described as
positive and self-perpetuating; that is, success-
ful performance generates higher efficacy, which
in turn leads to better performance outcomes
(Lindsley, Brass, & Thomas, 1995). For an execu-
tive (the agent) who must make strategic choices
on behalf of the firm, the concept of perceived
firm efficacy has relevance to goal setting, effort,
and the strategic choices they believe can be suc-
cessful (Bandura, 1977; Wood & Bandura, 1989).
For instance, if the agent perceives higher levels
of firm efficacy, they will be likely to set more
challenging strategic goals due to confidence
that these goals can be achieved (Bandura, 1997;
Earley, Northcraft, Lee, & Lituchy, 1990). Higher
perceived firm efficacy is also likely to inspire the
agent to persist in the pursuit of these goals, lead-
ing to superior performance outcomes (Lindsley
et al., 1995). Although the concept of efficacy
has been shown to influence decision making

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT