A Multistage Social Learning Model of the Influences of Family and Peers upon Adolescent Substance Abuse

Published date01 July 1988
Date01 July 1988
DOI10.1177/002204268801800301
Subject MatterArticle
The Journal of Drug Issues, 18(3),293-315, 1988
A MULTISTAGE SOCIAL LEARNING MODEL
OF THE INFLUENCES OF
FAMILY
AND
PEERS
UPON
ADOLESCENT SUBSTANCE ABUSE
Ronald L. Simons
Rand D. Conger
Leslie B. Whitbeck
Amultistage social learning model of parental and
peer influences on substance abuse is proposed.
Building upon the work
of
Bandura (1977) and
Patterson (1986), but also drawing from theory and
research on coping
and
value socialization, the
model provides an explanatory framework for many
of
the well-established empirical generalizations con-
cerning drug use
and
also generates a number
of
new
hypotheses. While the modelprovides an explanation
for initiation into substance use, it is primarily
concerned with identifying those factors which cause
adolescents to escalate their involvement with sub-
stances. Significantly, in the course
of
tracing the
etiology
of
heavy substance use, the model identifies
possible causal mechanisms related to two other
adolescent social problems, oiz., depression
and
delinquency,
and
specifies the mannerin which these
phenomena are related to substance use.
Resear ch on adolescent substance use
and
abuse supports
the
following
generalizations: a)
the
use of drugs is interrelated so
that
users of
anyone
type
are
more likely
than
nonusers to be users of any other type of drug
(Fishburne et al., 1980; Kandel, 1975; Kandel
et
al., 1978); b) usage
patterns
tend
to display developmental sequences progressing from
the
use of
the
legal drugs
alcohol
and
tobacco to
the
use ofillicit substances (Fisburne et al., 1980;Kandel et
al., 1978; Kandel
and
Yamaguchi, 1985; Miller,
1981);
c) there is a correlation
between life
stress
and
substance use (Shiffman
and
Wills, 1985);
and
d)
substance abuse is associated with weak bonds to family
and
school
and
strong
bonds to a deviant peer group (Bachman et aI., 1981; Brook et al., 1977, 1980;
Ronald
L.
Simons
is Professor of Sociologyat IowaState University.
Rand
D.
Conger
is Associate Dean for Research in
the. College of Family and Consumer Sciences at Iowa State University.
Leslie
B.
Whitbeck
is Assistant Professor of
~1.Ology
at Iowa State University, Ames, Iowa
50011.
This work wassupported byresearch
grant
DA05347·01 from the
atlonal Instituta on Drug Abuse.
Journal ofDrug Issues, Inc. 0022-0426/8&103/293-315 $1.00
293
SIMONS
Elliott et al., 1985; Huba et al., 1979 and 1980; Ginsberg and Greenley, 1978;
Jessor and Jessor, 1977; Orcutt,
1978).
While there is strong evidence for these
generalizations, researchers have yet to develop a theoretical model which will
explain and integrate these findings, specifying
the
causal ordering of
the
associations and explicating
their
relationship to each other.
During the 1960s and 1970s
strain
theories of adolescent deviance were quite
popular (Cohen, 1955; Cloward and Ohlin,
1960).
These approaches posit
that
lack
of access to conventional opportunities for success lead youth to join deviant peer
groups. Although
strain
theories provide an explanation for
the
relationship
between failure in school
and
involvement in a delinquent peer group, they do not
offer an explanationfor the other relationships cited above. Enthusiasm for
strain
theories has waned during recent years at least in
part
because of disconfirming
findings (Nettler, 1984;
VoId
and Bernard, 1986).
Presently, social control theory is probably
the
most popular model of adoles-
cent deviance. This theory maintains
that
youth engage in deviant behavior
because
their
ties to conventional social institutions, such as family and school,
are weak (Hirschi,
1969).
While there is ample support for
this
contention, the
perspective is not very helpful in explaining
the
relationships cited abovein
that
it
fails to identify conditions
that
lead to weak bonds (Elliott et al., 1985),it ignores
the
impact of deviant peers (Conger, 1980; Elliott et al., 1985),
and
it cannot
explain
the
development of motives conducive to the use of drugs (Elliott et al.,
1985).
In recent years, social learning theory has been suggested as an alternative
which addresses some of
the
limitations ofcontrol theory, while at
the
same time
remaining consistent with its obvious strengths (Akers, 1985; Conger,
1976).
By
focusing upon processes of modeling and reinforcement, social learning theory is
more explicit about those parenting factors which contribute to substance use,
and, unlike control theory, it predicts arelationship between drug use and
participation in a substance using peer group (Conger, 1980). Research based
upon this approach often explains more variance in substance use and abuse
than
that
explained by other perspectives (Akers et al.,
1979).
However,
in
the
form in
which it has been applied, the theory still leaves many questions unanswered. For
instance, while involvement with substance-using peers is a potent predictor of
drug abuse (Kandel, 1983), the theory does not explain why adolescents join
substance-using peer groups. This is a very important issue, both theoretically
and practically, as the answer might suggest approaches to
treatment
and
prevention. The theory also fails to account for
the
relationship between substance
use
and
both poor performance in school
and
indicators of stress, and it does not
specify
the
factors which cause progression from experimentation to regular use.
The present paper develops an expanded, more cognitive, social learning
explanation of patterns of heavy substance use. While
the
model identifies
processes accounting for initiation into substance use, its primary concern is
specifying
the
manner
in which parental
and
peer relationship factors cause
adolescents to escalate their use of alcohol and/or illicit drugs from experimenta-
tion to continuing abuse. Building upon
the
work ofBandura (1977) and Patterson
(1982),
but
also drawing from theory
and
research on coping and value socializa-
tion, the-model provides an explanation for
the
relationships described above
and
also generates anumber of new hypotheses.
294 JOURNAL OF DRUG ISSUES
A MULTISTAGE SOCIAL LEARNING MODEL
Any theory which purports to offer a comprehensive explanation of adolescent
substance abuse will of necessity be complex, containing several variables
and
reciprocal relationships. 'Ib reduce complexity,
the
proposed model focuses only
upon those direct and indirect
parent
and
peer influences related to substance
abuse for which there is considerable support. Even with this limitation, however,
the model remains complex simply illustrating
the
fact
that
patterns of drug use
are determined by many factors, and previous theories have been inadequate
because they have attempted to explain drug use through a two or three variable
process (Lettieri et al.,
1980).
The
Model
The expanded social learning model is presented in Figure 1. In an effort to
make
the
figure easier to read,
the
variables which represent the primary foci of
the
model-parenting
factors, peer group influences, and adolescent substance
abuse-are
put
in circles
rather
than
rectangles. Bold lines are used to identify
the four variables thought to directly effect substance abuse
and
dashes
rather
than
a solid line are employed to denote feedback loops or non-recursive effects.
The model begins with a
set
of parenting factors posited to impact upon various
aspects of adolescent psychosocial development. These developmental outcomes,
in turn, are linked to choice of peers, performance in school, initiation into
substance use, emotional disturbance, and, finally, a
pattern
of regular substance
use.
Figure 1.
AMultistage
Social
Learning
Model
of
Substance
Abuse
PARENTS
ADOLESCENTS
I I
L
~_-_B------,
~ ~
-I
---
1JJ
Sununer1988
295
SIMONS
Parenting Factors
As will become apparent in
the
discussion to
follow,
parenting factors are seen
as contributing both directly
and
indirectly to adolescent substance abuse. Given
the
central role attributed to parenting practices,
the
model begins by identifying
a
set
of factors thought to be important determinants of parenting style. Arrow 1
posits
that
adults are more likely to use an ineffective parenting style when they
were raised in a family characterized by faulty parenting. Parenting might be
classified as ineffective when it results in undesirable socialization outcomes (e.g.,
delinquency, psychopathology, non-compliance, etc.), Research has shown
that
faulty parenting may take the form of an inadequate parent-child relationship or
the use of deficient parenting techniques. Thus, parents can be ineffective either
because they fail to establish a warm, supportive relationship with
their
child,
and/or because they do not provide proper supervision, reinforcement,
and
discipline (Maccoby and Martin, 1983; Patterson, 1982; Rohner,
1986).
The
findings of two longitudinal studies (Edler et al., 1983; Huesmann
et
aI., 1983)
provide some support for the hypothesis
that
inadequate parenting practices tend
to be handed down from one generation to another.
Arrow 2 indicates
that
parents who are under stress, whether from work,
their
marriage, or some other source, tend to become less effective parents. Several
studies report
that
stress decreases
the
probability
that
a
parent
will use
efficacious childrearing techniques. Research has demonstrated
that
parents
under stress are less likely
than
non-stressed parents to nurture, monitor or
positively reinforce
their
children while they
are
more likely to use nagging,
scolding, hitting, and other punishment oriented responses (Conger et al., 1984;
Elder, 1974; Lahey et al., 1984; Patterson, 1983 and 1986).
Finally, consistent with the findings of several studies (Shiffman and Wills,
1985),
arrows 3 and 4 suggest
that
both environmental stress
and
inadequate
coping skills (i.e., excessive use of denial and avoidance
rather
than
direct action
strategies when stressful situations are encountered) contribute to parental
substance abuse, with parental substance abuse, in turn, impacting upon parent-
ing practices. Recently, Dishion et aI. (1985)reported
that
heavy substance-using
parents tended to display a parenting style characterized by ineffective practices
such as low involvement and inept monitoring.
Psychosocial Outcomes
Arrows 6 through 10 focus upon
the
impact of parenting factors upon adoles-
cent psychosocial development. Arrow 6 indicates
that
an adolescent's social skills
(i.e., interpersonal competencies associated with listening, compromise, assert-
iveness, empathy, accepting authority, relating to
the
opposite sex, etc.) are
determined, in part, by the childrearing techniques employed by his or
her
parents. Patterson (1982 and 1986) has shown how a coercive family system
develops in response to a parenting style characterized by little monitoring and
positive reinforcement, coupled with high rates ofpunishmentand rejection. Such
asystem is marked by high levels of mutual punishment as
the
primary means by
which family members attemptto control one another's behavior (Conger,
1981).
In
this type of environment,
the
child learns to use aggressive, coercive methods to
influence others while more socially appropriate interpersonal skills are not
modeled or reinforced.
296
JOURNAL OF DRUG ISSUES
A MULTISTAGE SOCIAL LEARNING MODEL
The affective
nature
of
the
parent-child relationship also influences social skill
development. A loving,
nurturing
relationship with
parents
is posited to be
the
primary vehicle through which chidren
learn
to care, to give, to compromise, to
trust,
and
to risk showing
their
feelings
and
weaknesses to others (Maccoby
and
Martin, 1983; Hoffman, 1983). Hence, youth who are socialized in an atmosphere
ofparental neglect
and
rejection might be expected to lack
the
interpersonal skills
necessary to form close relationships with others. Such children are likely to be
self-centered, insensitive to
the
feelings of others,
and
perhaps socially with-
drawn.
As noted above, research
has
shown
that
both delinquency
and
substance
abuse
are
associated with poor school performance
and
involvement in a deviant
peer group.
Strain
theories account for these relationships by positing
that
youth
who fail in school join delinquent gangs in
their
search for alternative avenues to
success
and
recognition (Cohen, 1955; Cloward
and
Ohlin, 1960). Research
findings, however, provide only weak support for this perspective (Nettler, 1984;
VoId
and
Bernard, 1986). Control theory (Hirschi, 1969) holds
that
adolescents
often perform poorly in school because they have failed to acquire values
supporting commitment to education, albeit
the
theory offers no explanation for
the
motives
that
lead individuals to participate in deviant peer groups.
Building upon
the
contentions of control theory,
the
social learning model
presented in Figure 1suggests
that
the
value socialization outcomes which foster
poor school performance also encourage involvement with deviant peers. This
prediction was derived by combining Bandura's (1977) emphasis upon
the
pro-
cesses of self-reinforcement
and
self-regulation with findings regarding
the
importance of parents as reference groups for adolescents' future-oriented goals
(Brittain, 1963; Sebald, 1986; Wilks, 1986).
In describing
the
socialization process,
Bandura
(1977)contends that:
Parents cannot always be present to guide
their
children's behavior. Suc-
cessful socialization requires gradual substitution of symbolic
and
internal
controls for external sanctions
and
demands (p. 43)
....
Theories
that
explain
human
behavior as solely
the
product of external rewards
and
punishments present atruncated image of people. . . . Behavior is com·
monly performed in
the
absence ofimmediate external reinforcement. Some
activities are maintained by anticipated consequences,
but
most
are
under
self-reinforcement control. In
this
process, people
set
certain
standards
of
behavior for themselves
and
respond to
their
own actions in self-reinforcing
or self-punishing ways (p, 129).
In contrast to lower level organisms,
the
human
being is not a slave to his or
her
external environment. Rather, people
tend
to acquire performance
standards
which they use in regulating, evaluating,
and
self-reinforcing
their
actions.
Indeed, one way of defining maturity is
the
extent to which
an
individual's
behavior is a function of
internal
rather
than
external contingencies.
Bandura
uses
the
label "performance standard" to refer to
the
symbolic criteria
which individuals employ in regulating
and
evaluating
their
behavior. Sociologists
apply
the
term
"value" to such performance criteria. According to Rokeach's
(1973:5)widely cited definition, a value is "an enduring belief
that
a specific mode
Summer 1988 297
SIMONS
of conduct or end-state of existence is personally preferable to an opposite or
converse mode of conduct or end-state of existence." As the definitions suggest,
there are two types of values: terminal values which have to do with end-states of
existence (e.g., world at peace, equality, material success, etc.) and instrumental
values which have to do with modes of conduct (e.g., courage, honesty, etc.),
Terminal values are conceived as more centrally located in a person's beliefsystem
than
instrumental values, and both are considered to be more centrally located or
fundamental
than
beliefs, attitudes, or opinions about specific objects or situa-
tions (Rokeach,
1968).
Research findings suggest
that
youth are less likely to drift into delinquency
when they are committed to
the
work ethic
(Agnew,
1984),have high educational
and occupational aspirations (Hirschi, 1969), and are religious (Albrecht et al.,
1977; Burkett and White, 1984; Elifson et al., 1983;
Jensen
and Erickson, 1979).
Moreover, support for the idea
that
long-term value commitments are somehow
involved in
the
etiology of substance use is provided by two recent studies by
Kandel and Yamaguchi showing
that
users of marijuana postpone marriage and
parenthood longer
than
nonusers (Yamaguchi
and
Kandel, 1985) and
that
heavy
substance users display higher self-initiatedjob mobility
than
others (Kandel and
Yamaguchi,
1987).
This raises
the
question of
the
source offuture-oriented values.
Several studies have found
that
adolescents utilize their parents as a reference
group for long-term, future-oriented values (career, religion, money, etc.) while
they
tum
to their peers when forming more immediate, short-term values (e.g.,
dress, hobbies, dating, etc.) (Brittain, 1963; Sebald, 1986; Wilks,
1986).
These
findings suggest
that
youth tend to adopt educational and occupational aspira-
tions, a concern with marriage and parenthood, an orientation toward work, and
religious commitments which reflect the views of
their
parents. This hypothesis is
depicted by arrow 6. The arrow indicates
that
when parents possess strong value
commitments in
the
areas of work, family, education, occupation, and religion,
their children are likely to develop similar commitments; and when parents lack
value commitments in these areas, it is likely
that
their children will develop a
similar lack of concern with such values.
It
would
follow
that
the
concerns of youth who lack a commitment to future-
oriented values will be limited to more immediate, hedonistic goals. It will be
argued below
that
such persons are likely to be attracted to deviant peer groups
which emphasize excitement
and
immediate gratification. There is evidence
that
delinquent youth are more committed
than
conforming adolescents to subterra-
nean values of the society (Matza, 1964) involving search for excitement and
pursuit ofpleasure (Cernkovich, 1978a and 1978b).1b a large degree these values
probably develop in deviant peer groups as
the
members, lacking long-term value
commitments, reinforce each other's pursuit of immediate goals. In some cases,
however, parents, through their actions and talk, may model a commitment to
subterranean values. Thus, parents may foster in
their
children a concern with
excitement and immediate gratification in two ways: a) by failing to socialize long-
term values and, thereby, facilitating their child's participation in peer groups
where subterranean, hedonistic values are accepted and reinforced; and b) by
directly socializing their children, either through words or actions, into subterra-
nean value orientations.
Assuming
that
parents are committed to future-oriented values, arrow 8
298 JOURNAL OF DRUG ISSUES
A MULTISTAGE SOCIAL LEARNING MODEL
specifies more precisely
the
conditions under which
their
children
are
likely to
adopt these commitments. Parents
must
employ effective childrearing techniques
if they
are
'to pass
their
values on to
their
children. Parents
must
model (through
their actions
and
their
talk)
and
reinforce future-oriented values concerned with
education
and
vocation, work, family
and
religion if
their
offspring are to
internalize these standards. Social learning theory would suggest
that
it is not
enough for parents to hold values in these areas; they need to use effective
parenting skills if these value commitments
are
to be passed on to
their
children.
Part
of effective parenting is establishing positive affective ties with one's
children. Youth
are
unlikely to adopt
the
values of
their
parents when they feel
rejected by them. Research on modeling shows
that
individuals
are
most likely to
emulate
the
actions of persons they hold in high esteem and/or who
are
potent
sources of reinforcement (Bandura, 1977; Parry
and
Furukawa, 1980). Thus,
parents who possess a close, warm relationship with
their
offspring are most
apt
to influence
their
children's values (Conger, 1976; Feather, 1975).
Arrow 9 indicates arelationship between
the
coping skills modeled by parents
and
the
coping techniques employed by
their
adolescent children. Social learning
theory would posit
that
youth
learn
strategies for coping with stress and tension,
in part, from
their
adult caretakers. They learn to use alcohol, to withdraw, to
pray, to ask for support,
and
the like, by observing the behavior of
their
parents
(Yost
and
Mines, 1985). There is some evidence
that
both adult
and
adolescent
substance abusers lack effective coping skills (Pentz, 1985; Rouse et al., 1973;
Stone et al., 1985; Timmer et al., 1985; Wills, 1985). The model proposes
that
adolescents who become substance abusers possess a limited repertoire of coping
skills,
and
that
they tend to respond to stressors with strategies involving denial,
avoidance, and/or distraction. As arrow 9suggests, we expect
that
this same
limited range of responses is evident in
the
coping strategies employed by
their
parents.
Asdescribed by arrow 10, an adolescent's level of social skill development is also
considered to be a determinant of his or
her
coping pattern. Individuals who have
difficulty forming close friendships, who
are
uncomfortable expressing
their
feelings, or who
tend
to be either
under
or overly assertive will be unable to use
certain coping strategies. They will find it difficult, for example, to appropriately
confront people serving as a source of stress, or to mobilize social support (i.e.,
advice, assistance, or reassurance) in response to a stressor. Importantly, several
stUdies have shown such coping strategies to be among
the
most effective
approaches to dealing with life stress (Kessler et al., 1985).
Finally, arrows 11,12,
and
13 identify
the
factors thought to be important in
causing early initiation into substance use. Several studies have shown
that
the
earlier ayoungster begins substance use,
the
greater
the
probability
that
he or
she will develop a substance abuse problem (Fleming et al., 1982; Kandel, 1976;
Kaplan et al., 1984; Kaplan et al., 1986; Robins
and
Przybeck, 1985; Robins
and
Murphy, 1967). Further, there is strong evidence
that
involvement in substance
abuse tends to follow a
pattern
where experimentation with alcohol precedes
trying marijuana,
and
using marijuana occurs prior to experimentation with
other illicit drugs (Kandel
and
Yamaguchi, 1985).Thus,
the
data
indicate
that
age
?f initiation into substance use,
and
the
substance is usually alcohol, is somehow
lnvolved in
the
development of a heavy substance use pattern.
Sununer
1988
299
SIMONS
The present model contends
that
early initiation into substance use is a
function of three factors: First, early initiation is likely when
an
adolescent's
parents model regular substance use. Consistent with this prediction, Kandel et
aI. (1978) report
that
heavy substance use by parents is related to early alcohol
experimentation by
their
children. Second, adolescents are more
apt
to experi-
ment with substances at a young age when their parents utilize inadequate
parenting techniques. Avariety of studies indicate
that
there is an increased
probability
that
teens will drift into delinquent behavior of all types, including
substance use, when parents fail to effectively monitor, reward, and punish
the
behavior of their children (Baumrind, 1985; Patterson, 1982; Patterson and
Stouthammer-Loeber, 1984;Penning
and
Barnes, 1982; Reilly,1979),while results
from a panel study by Kaplan et aI. (1986) show
that
adolescents are more
apt
to
experiment with drugs at an early age when their parents fail to
set
clear rules.
Third, it is posited
that
early initiation into substance use is encouraged by low
commitment to future oriented values and/or astrong commitment to subterra-
nean, hedonistic values. Subterranean values concerning excitement and new
experiences are likely to make experimentation with substances attractive, while
low concern for future-oriented goals reduces
the
risk associated with such
behavior.
Peers, School Performance,
and
Emotional Distress
The remaining relationships in Figure 1 focus upon
the
consequences of the
psychosocial factors
just
considered. Arrows 14 through 17 posit
that
a young
person's choice of peer group is influenced by
his/her
social skills, values, age of
initiation into substance use, and
the
parenting techniques utilized by his/her
parents. The social skills prediction is supported by sociometric research on
children. Several studies have found
that
youth who are overly assertive (arguing,
bragging, fighting) and/or withdrawn (shy, uninvolved, sulking, excluding of
others) tend to be rejected by
their
peers who view such behaviors as self-centered
and noxious (Asher and Renshaw,
1981;
Dodge,1983;Putallaz
and
Gottman,
1981;
Snyder et al., 1986).And, having been rejected by peers with appropriate social
skills, there is evidence
that
children with deficient social skills often drift into
association with one another (Hartup, 1983; Snyder
et
al.,
1986).
Given
their
socialization histories, such youngsters might be expected to be accepting of each
other's self-centered, coercive
pattern
of interaction. Thus, adolescents who have
not learned to be polite, tactful, sensitive, and compromising in their interactions
with others are likely to end up participating in a deviant peer group where
their
interpersonal style is accepted.
As noted earlier, there is strong evidence
that
parental mismanagementfosters
aggressive children (Patterson, 1986)
and
that
parental
rejection
and
an
authoritarian approach to parenting often cause children to be shy and under-
assertive (Maccoby
and
Martin, 1983).Hence, in
the
proposed model two types of
social skill deficits are posited as a consequence of inadequate
parenting-
aggressiveness and
shyness-and
these two forms of interpersonal deviance are
seen as contributing to rejection by more popular, socially appropriate peers and
subsequent drift into association with other deviant, rejected kids. Note
that
this
line ofreasoning offers an explanation for
the
finding by Kellam
and
his associates
300 JOURNAL OF DRUG ISSUES
A MULTISTAGE SOCIAL LEARNING MODEL
that
shyness and/or aggressiveness in first grade predicts substance abuse during
adolescence (Kellam and Brown, 1982).
While
the
indirect effect of parenting upon choice of peer group through social
skills
has
been noted, parenting factors are also posited to have a direct effect
upon
the
type of peer group in which an adolescent becomes involved. Arrow 15
suggests
that
effective parents
are
able to have some impact upon
their
child's
choice of friends. Parental monitoring
and
discipline can affect an adolescent's
access to and association with deviant peers. Studies by Patterson
and
his
colleagues have shown this to be
the
case (Patterson
and
Dishion, 1985; Snyder et
al., 1986).And,
the
quality of
the
relationship between
parent
and
child impacts
upon a parent's ability to influence his/her child's friendship choices in
that
youngsters
are
likely to care about
and
be responsive to parental opinions about
their peers when parents represent highly reinforcing figures. Conversely, youth
are likely to be unresponsive to parental concerns about friendship choices when
the parent-child relationship lacks warmth
and
affection (Conger, 1976).
Arrow 16 suggests
that
early substance use also influences
the
type of peer
group with which
an
adolescent is likely to become involved. As noted above,
the
earlier aperson experiments with substances
the
more likely it is
that
he or she
will go on to become a heavy substance user. Although several studies have noted
this relationship (Kandel et al., 1978; Robins
and
Pryzbeck, 1985),an explanation
has yet to be developed concerning
the
manner
in which early initiation contrib-
utes to heavy substance use. Robins
and
Przybeck (1985) have shown
that
the
relationship cannot be explained by
the
hypothesis
that
psychopathology causes
some individuals to be both early initiators
and
subsequent substance abusers.
The presentmodel posits
that
the
effect is mediated through type of peergroup.
Youngsters who use substances (usually alcohol) at
an
early age are likely to be
perceived as deviant,
and
are
apt
to be ostracized by conforming peers. As a result
ofthis rejection, early experimenters might be expected to drift into deviant peer
groUps, i.e., peer groups which accept
their
unconventional behavior. Consistent
with this prediction, four longitudinal studies show
that
beginning to use drugs
increases
the
probability
that
one will become
part
of a drug using peer group
(Cohen, 1977; Kandel, 1978; Kaplan et al., 1984; Ginsberg
and
Greenley, 1978).
And, as will be argued below,joining adeviant peer group, is a major contributor
to sustained involvement with drugs.
Finally,
an
adolescent's values are likely to be important in
the
selection of a
peer group as studies have repeatedly found
that
people form friendships with
persons who possess values similar to
their
own (Arrow 17). Individuals
are
attracted to
and
reinforce one another to
the
extent
that
they are committed to
similar values
and
lines of action (Hartup, 1983).
It
was argued above
that
adolescents will display a value hiatus, or anomie, with regard to future-oriented
values (e.g., work ethic, educational
and
occupational aspirations, religious
commitment) if
their
parents lack such value commitments, if
their
parents
are
unaffectionate
and
rejecting, and/or if
their
parents
fail to utilize effective
?arenting techniques. Lacking long-term value commitments,
the
concern of such
Individuals is limited to
rather
immediate goals
and
interests. In other cases,
parents may actually model
and
reinforce subterranean, hedonistic values.
Regardless, adolescents concerned with more present-oriented goals
and
inter-
ests
are
likely to be more responsive
than
youngsters with future-oriented values
Summer
1988
301
SIMONS
to reinforcers operating in the immediate environment. As aresult, adolescents
who have not been socialized into long-range value commitments are likely to join
peer groups which emphasize immediate, hedonistic pursuits. Moreover,as arrow
28 indicates, when
the
members of a group are committed to fun and excitement
while being unconcerned about costs which these pursuits might have for future-
oriented goals, they are likely to experiment with delinquent activities.
Arrows 18, 19, and 20 indicate
that
value commitments, parenting techniques,
and type of peer group impact upon performance in school. Youngsters who have
not been socialized into future-oriented values are not likely to
put
much effort
into school; lax or rejecting parents fail to monitor
and
support completion of
homework assignments (Dishion et al., 1984); and, young persons involved with
peers oriented to
the
pursuit of immediate goals and the use of a self-centered
interpersonal style are likely to be frustrated by
the
conduct and homework
requirements of school (Patterson,
1986).
Social learning theory suggests
that
a person's self-concept is determined by
the feedback he/she gets about his/her performances (Bandura, 1977). Therefore,
poor performance in school might be expected to reduce an adolescent's self-
esteem (arrow 23), a finding
that
has been reported in several studies (O'Malley
and Bachman, 1979). Arrows 21 and 22 posit
that
social skills and parenting
practices also influence self-esteem. As discussed earlier, socially incompetent
youth (whether too aggressive or too shy) tend to be rejected by
their
more socially
adequate peers. In addition to pushing
the
youngster toward membership in a
deviant peer group,
this
rejection should lower self-esteem. There is strong
evidence
that
parenting practices impact upon a young person's self-esteem.
Studies have shown
that
parental permissiveness, lowinvolvement, rejection, and
punitiveness tend to diminish ayouths sense of worth
(Bowlby,
1980; Bachman,
1982; Coopersmith, 1967; Greenberg et al., 1983; Rosenberg, 1965).
Arrows 24 through 27 identify factors which serve as important causes of
emotional disturbance for an adolescent. Parental rejection, deficient coping
skills, low self-esteem, and a lack of long-term values
and
goals (i.e., anomie) are
posited to increase a young person's level of tension, anxiety,
and
depression.
There are several studies which suggest
that
these factors are important
contributors to emotional disturbance during
the
adolescent years (Charteir and
Ranieri, 1984; Hodges and Siegel, 1985; Simons and Murphy, 1985). Contempo-
rary theories emphasize the way in which perceptions of helplessness (Seligman
et al., 1979; Peterson and Seligman, 1985)
and
an attitude of pessimism and
hopelessness concerning oneself, other people, and
the
future lead to depression
and anxiety (Beck and Rush, 1978; Peterson
and
Seligman, 1985). Adolescents
who are rejected by
their
parents and have deficient coping skills and low self-
esteem might be expected to suffer feelings of inefficacy and helplessness; and
youth lacking in fundamental, long-term values are likely to be vulnerable to
feelings of meaninglessness and purposelessness regarding
the
future.
Escalation to Heavy Substance Use
Weare now ready to consider
the
factors posited to directly contribute to heavy
substance use. Note
that
the model distinguishes initiation from heavy use. Many
ofthe inconsistent findings in
the
drug use literature may be a function of the fact
that
researchers often fail to distinguish experimentation from regular use. Most
302 JOURNAL OF DRUG
ISSUES
A MULTISTAGE SOCIAL LEARNING MODEL
young people consume alcohol at some point during
their
high school years,
and
a
substantial proportion
try
illicit drugs during this period of
their
lives (Baumrind,
1985; Kandel, 1983). As noted above, early experimentation is thought to be a
function of parental modeling of substance use, ineffective parenting skills,
and
a
commitment to
subterranean
rather
than
future oriented values. Stated differ-
ently, kids whose values emphasize immediate gratification
and
whose
parents
use substances
and
exercise little supervision
and
control, are likely to experi-
ment with substances at an early age. However,
the
vast majority of kids who
experiment with drugs do not go on to become regular users. The model suggests
four factors which increase
the
probability
that
experimentation will escalate to
heavy usage of alcohol and/or illicit drugs. The four factors are
the
substance use
pattern of parents, type of peer group, psychopathology,
and
coping skills.
Prior to discussing these factors, it should be noted
that
some individuals
contend
that
the
most parsimonious explanation for substance abuse is
that
it is
an operant behavior developed in response to
the
biologically reinforcing proper-
ties of
the
chemicals involved (Council on Scientific Affairs, 1982).This contention
is consistent with studies showing
that
animals quickly
learn
to display high
response rates in order to obtain many of
the
illicit drugs used by humans, often
selecting these substances over other potent reinforcers such as food
and
water
(Young
and
Herling, 1986). Studies of humans, however, suggest
that
drug abuse
cannot be explained so easily.
As noted above, unlike laboratory animals, most
humans
who experiment with
a Substance do not go on to become regular users. Further, studies of patients self-
administering analgesics for post-operative pain, d-amphetamines for weight
control, or psychotropic drugs for psychiatric distress show
that
people
are
generally quite conservative in
their
usage, often imbibing less
than
the
physician
recommended levels (See Henningfield et al., 1986). Similarly, laboratory experi-
ments have shown
that
when given
the
choice
humans
tend
to select placebos
substantially more often
than
d-amphetamine,
and
that
preference for
the
drug
actually declines over trials (Johanson
and
Uhlenhuth, 1980; Uhlenhuth et al.,
1981).
Thus, it appears
that
it is a mistake to view drug abuse as caused by
invariable physiologic consequences of drugs which compel
the
individual to
take
the substance. Rather, it seems
that
drug reinforcement is a variable phenomenon
which is influenced by special characteristics of users
and
their
life situations
(Martin, 1983; Nicholi, 1983). As mentioned above,
the
present model identifies
four factors thought to augment
the
reinforcing value of substance use. By
operating to increase
the
reinforcement associated with drugs, these factors serve
to escalate
the
degree to which substances
are
used'!
The first factor involves
the
substance use
pattern
modeled by
parents
(arrow
30).There is evidence
that
adolescents are more
apt
to engage in heavy use
if
their
parents display a
pattern
of regular alcohol and/or drug use (Barnes et al., 1986;
~ndel
et
al., 1978; Wegscheider,
1981).
Through
their
behavior,
parents
convey
Information concerning when substances
are
to be used
and
the
quantities in
which they
are
to be taken. Social learning theory would predict
that
adolescents
are more
apt
to use substances to relax or to cope with stress,
and
are
more likely
to imbibe frequently
and
in large quantities, if such behaviors
are
modeled by
their parents. Further, several studies show
that
individuals
learn
expectancies
about
the
psychological
and
behavioral effects of a drug
and
that
these expectan-
SUtnmer 1988 303
SIMONS
cies color their response to
the
substance (Adesso,
1985).
Presumably, the
formulation of these expectancies begins in
the
home as the youngster observes
his/her parents talk about and utilize various substances.
Anumber of studies show
that
over time youth tend to adopt values and
behavior patterns which are similar to those of
their
friends (Hartup, 1983),
and
consistent with this finding, there is overwhelming evidence
that
adolescent
substance users tend to have friends who use drugs (Kandel, 1983). Social
learning theory would contend
that
this relationship exists as drug-using peers,
through
their
talk
and behavior, create expectancies concerning drug effects
and
then
reinforce individuals for taking drugs
and
acting in a
manner
consistent
with the expected effects. Arrow 31 indicates this hypothesis.
In addition to
the
influences exerted by parents and peers, arrow 32 hypoth-
esizes
that
emotional distress (i.e., anxiety and/or depression) sets
the
stage for
other types of reinforcers to come into play. Studies with both adults
and
adolescents suggest
that
stress, anxiety, and depression are related to substance
abuse (Alterman, 1985; Mirin, 1984; Robins and Przybeck,
1985).
Based upon
social learning theory, the present model posits
that
emotional distress may
contribute to
the
reinforcing value ofsubstance use in one of two ways.
First, intoxicants can be used to enhance one's sense of mastery and to produce
new repertoires of behavior. For example, based upon learned expectancies and
experience with some drug, individuals lacking social skills may find
that
use of
the
substance makes them feel less self-conscious
and
more socially capable.
Similarly, persons low in self-esteem
and
self-efficacymight discover
that
certain
drugs make them feel more able and confident. In such instances
the
use of a
substance takes on reinforcing value because it establishes
the
conditions (i.e., it
becomes a discriminative stimulus) for experiencing other reinforcers.
Second, substance use may act as a negative reinforcer for individuals
undergoing emotional distress. This is particularly likely to be the case for
adolescents manifesting psychopathology in
the
form of depression and/or anx-
iety.Such distress appears to have increased dramatically among teens during
the
last
twenty-five years,
and
now is relatively common in this age group (Chartier
and
Ranieri,
1984).
Once initiated into drug use a young person may find
that
certain drugs reduce feelings oftension, anxiety, and depression. Thus, drugs may
be used as a form of self-medication in an effort to control the emotions created by
stressful situations.
Studies of adults have found an association between emotional distress,
particularly depression,
and
substance use (Day and Leonard, 1985). While
research to date has not established
the
causal direction of
this
relationship,
the
present model posits
that
the
primary effect is from emotional upset to substance
abuse, with use of
the
substance being maintained through
the
process ofnegative
reinforcement. Support for this causal claim is provided by laboratory studies
with animals (MacLennan and Maier, 1983)
and
humans
(Uhlenhuth et al., 1981)
showing
that
both select drugs more often during conditions of stress and
dysphoria,
and
by
the
results of a recent panel study of adolescents which found
that
psychological distress at the time of initiation predicted escalation of
substance use (Kaplan et al.,
1986).
Some have argued
that
while drug experimen-
tation is motivated by a concern for increasing positive affect, regular users are
motivated by a concern for reducing negative affect (McKennel and .Thomas, 1967;
304
JOURNAL
OF
DRUG
ISSUES
A MULTISTAGE SOCIAL LEARNING MODEL
Ashton and Stepney,
1982).
As should be evident, this hypothesis is a
part
of
the
proposed model.
In addition to parental modeling, peer reinforcement, and emotional distress,
the model indicates
that
substance use is influenced by a person's coping skills
(arrow 33). Marlatt (1979) has argued
that
problem drinking is a means ofcoping
among people who feel inadequate about
their
capacities to cope with stress.
Recently, Wills
and
Shiffman (1985)have made a similar contention with regard
tosubstance use in general. They note
that
research has established an intercor-
relation between cigarette smoking, heavy drinking, and other substance use
among teens. These findings, they contend, suggest
that
similar mechanisms
underlie
the
use of different substances. They go on to propose
that
these similar
mechanisms "lie in the domain of general coping skills" (Wills and Shiffman,
1985:18).
Several of the papers in
their
edited volume report findings supporting
the hypothesis
that
substance users tend to suffer from a limited range of coping
skills (Pentz, 1985; Stone et al., 1985; Timmer et al., 1985; Wills,
1985).
As
part
of the present model, we posit
that
regular users rely largely upon
coping strategies involving denial, avoidance, and/or distraction. Reliance upon
such strategies is likely to contribute to substance abuse in several ways: a)
substance use is consistent with or can be an integral
part
of coping responses
designed to deny, avoid, or forget a stressor; b) substance use will have greater
reinforcing value for persons who lack other methods of coping with stress
than
it
will for individuals who have mastered a wide range of coping behaviors (Lawson
et al., 1983); and c) coping strategies involving denial, avoidance and distraction
are often not very effective methods for dealing with stressful situations (Pearlin
and Schooler, 1978), serving in
the
long
run
to intensify stress and the need to
utilize these coping strategies in a more extreme fashion. Thus, arrow 31actually
signifies three hypotheses concerning
the
manner
in which coping strategies
Contribute to regular substance use.
Research concerned with
the
relationship between coping skills and drug use
have employed either of two models. The stress/coping model conceptualizes
substance use as a compensatory or coping behavior adopted in response to stress.
Consistent with this view,Aneshensel and Huba (1983) found that, at leastin
the
short run, young people who used drugs experienced a decrease in anxiety and
depression. The stress/response model contends
that
drug use serves to increase
rather
than
alleviate stress over
the
long-term. The model assumes
that
heavy
drug use inhibits individuals from learning or practicing more adaptive coping
alternatives (Wrubel et al.,
1981).
This hypothesis is supported by Aneshensel and
Huba's (1983) finding
that
continued alcohol use tended to increase depression
and related behaviors.
The proposed model incorporates both perspectives. Thus far we have described
the stress coping component of the model. However, arrows 34 through 40 specify
the mannerin which regularsubstance use functions over time to intensify stress.
The arrows indicate
that
heavy use tends to limit coping
and
social skill
development, increase the probability of involvement with deviant peers, and
exacerbate family stress and conflict.
It
also serves to undermine performance in
school which further diminishes self-esteem and intensifies family stress. As
the
model shows, all of these feedback loops operate, at least indirectly, to augment
stress
and
substance use.
SlUnmer1988
305
SIMONS
Implications
In recent years many researchers have lamented
the
paucity of theory in the
area
ofsubstance use and abuse. Attempts to build theory usually fall short in one
of two ways. Either
the
theorists emphasize only one or two variables and
therefore oversimplify what is undoubtedly a very complex process; or they
developmodels consisting of an eclectic collection of variables which are not tied to
each other in any theoretically integrated fashion. The result is
that
while
research has established anumber ofcorrelates ofdrug use, no theoretical model
has been developed which specifies
the
causal ordering of these associations
and
explicates
their
relationship to each other. The present paper was an attempt to
show how social learning theory might be employed to build such a model.
The proposed model is complex.
However,
afteryears ofresearch on
the
topic, it
is
apparent
that
the
etiology of drug use, like most other
human
behaviors, is a
complex process involving a sequence of many factors, with these factors often
influencing each other in a reciprocal fashion. Hence, if a model is to identify the
important features of
the
etiology of substance use, it
must
of necessity include
several variables, be processual, and allow for reciprocal determination.
While there is strong empirical support for elements of the model, many of
the
relationships are offered as hypotheses to be tested. Undoubtedly, subsequent
research will show
parts
of the model to be inaccurate or in need of revision. The
value of the model, however, is
that
it uses a single theory, social learning theory,
to tie together and make predictions concerning a number of phenomena thought
to be important in the development of substance use and abuse. Significantly, in
the course of tracing the etiology of regular substance use, the model identifies
some possible determinants of two other adolescent social problems, viz., depres-
sion and delinquency,
and
specifies the manner in which these phenomena are
related to substance use.
With regard to the latter of these problems, some have argued
that
the
relationship between delinquency and substance use is spurious (Elliott and
Ageton, 1976; Johnston et al., 1978) while others have contended
that
minor
delinquency causes both drug use and serious delinquency (Clayton,
1981).
The
proposed model suggests
that
the
correlation between
the
two is a function of
the
fact
that
both are caused by membership in a peer group which combines lack of
commitment to future-oriented values with a focus upon subterranean, hedonistic
values.
Another strength of the model is
that
it not only describes the processes
whereby both parental
and
peer factors contribute to high levels of substance use,
but
also identifies
the
manner
in which parenting factors, both directly
and
indirectly through
their
impact upon adolescent values and social skills, may
influence an adolescent's choice of peer group. While scores of studies show
that
membership in a deviant peer group contributes to involvement in delinquency,
including substance use, extant theories of social deviance fail to adequately
identify those factors which cause a young person to join or be recruited into such
a peer group. Building upon
the
work of Patterson
(1986),
the
present model posits
an explanation for the process whereby ayouth becomes involved with deviant
peers.
The model is an attempt to identify
the
major factors operating in
the
etiology
of adolescent substance abuse.
It
should be noted, however,
that
the
adolescent
306 JOURNAL OF DRUG ISSUES
AMULTISTAGE SOCIAL LEARNING MODEL
period covers several years with early adolescence differing in many respects from
late adolescence.
It
seems likely
that
many of the factors specified in
the
model
exert more influence during certain phases of the adolescent period
than
at
others. For instance, Snyder
et
al. (1986) report
that
deficient social skills is
strongly related to involvement in a deviant peer group during early adolescence
but
has less of an impact during late adolescence when previously inadequate
youth have become more socially skilled and use coercion less frequently. And, it
seems probable
that
parenting practices have more impact during early adoles-
cence while the influence of peers increases throughout the period. A complete
theory of adolescent drug abuse would need to specify the manner in which
the
impact of various causal factors wax and wane as a youth matures.
Should research corroborate the major components of
the
model, it would
suggest anumber of treatment implications. Presently, most treatment programs
employ individual
and
group counseling to help adolescents identify the destruc-
tive consequences of theirsubstance use, and family therapy to redefine members'
boundaries and roles and to establish constructive communication patterns. The
proposed model would suggest
that
in addition to these activities, individual
treatment should focus upon teaching future-oriented values, social skills,
and
strategies for coping with stress, while family therapy should strive to increase
parents' nurturance
and
parenting skills, their range of coping strategies, and
their emphasis upon future-oriented values.
Prevention programs are usually concerned with changing attitudes about
substance use. The proposed model would emphasize the importance of fostering
future-oriented value commitments and of increasing the social and coping skills
of youngsters, while lowering family stress
and
facilitating the use of effective
parenting techniques. While
the
model provides no guidance concerning how
these goals might be accomplished, it points to
the
need to broaden the scope of
present programs.
NOTES
1. Note
that
self-esteem is not posited to have a direct effect upon initiation into or
escalation of
s~bstance
use. In the present model, self-esteem contributes to substance
abuse only indirectly through its impact upon psychological distress. This is contrary to
Kaplan's (1980 and 1982) contention
that
adolescents low on self-esteem lose
their
moti-
vation to engage in conventional behavior
and
strive to obtain
status
and recognition by
engaging in deviant behavior, including substance use. While Kaplan (1975, 1976 and 1977)
finds
that
self-rejection predicts subsequent involvement in deviant behavior, other recent
panel studies have found little or no support for this hypothesis (McCarthy and Hoge, 1984;
Wells and Rankin, 1983).
Recently, Kaplan et a!' (1986) have maintained
that
methodological deficiencies have
prevented researchers from finding a strong relationship between self-derogation and
deviant behavior. They contend that, in addition to measuring global self-esteem, an
adequate measure ofself-rejection
must
include perceptions ofdevaluation by parents, peers
and teachers. Further, they suggest
that
the effect of self-rejection upon deviant behavior is
mediated by its impact upon an adolescent's disposition or motivation to engage in deviant
behavior. In support of these arguments, data
are
presented which show a strong association
between disposition to deviance
and
actual deviant behavior.
Their arguments and LISREL analysis
are
problematic, however, in
that
their
multiple
indicator measure ofself-rejection includes three items focusing upon parental rejection and
Summer
1988 307
SIMONS
four items concerned with commitment to unconventional values, while their measure of
disposition to deviance consists of two scalee containing items concerning disaffection with
the conventional order and commitment to subterranean values. Including social rejection
as a component ofself-rejection makes the results hard to interpret. Rather than suggesting
arelationship between self-rejection
and
deviant behavior, their findings might simply be
showing:1)
that
parental rejection influences the extent to which an adolescent internalizes
conventional moral values, a finding well established in the socialization literature
(Maccoby
and Martin, 1983),or 2)
that
peer rejection is associated with involvement in a deviant peer
group and commitment to unconventional values (Patterson,
1986).
The relationship
between self-rejection and disposition to deviance might also be a function of the fact
that
the measures for these two variables demonstrate partial overlap as both contain items
concerned with commitment to subterranean values.
REFERENCES
Adesso,V.J.
1985 Cognitive factors in alcohol and drug use. In M. Galizio and S. A. Maisto (eds.),
Determinants
of
Substance Abuse: Biological, Psychological,and Environmental
Factors. New
York:
Plenum.
Agnew, R.
1984 The work ethic and delinquency. Sociological
&us.
17:337-346.
Akers,R. L.
1985 Deviant Behavior: A Social Learning Approach. 3rd. ed., Belmont, California:
Wadsworth.
Akers, R. L., M. D. Krohn, L. Lanza-Kanduce and M. J. Radosevich
1979 Social learning and deviant behavior: A specific test of a general theory.
American Sociological Review 44:636-655.
Albrecht, S. L., B. A. Chadwick and D. S. Alcorn
1977 Religiosity and deviance: Application of an attitude-behavior contingent consis-
tency model.Journal
of
the Scientific
Study
of
Religion 16:263-274.
Alterman, A. I.
1985 Substance Abuse
and
Psychopathology. New
York:
Plenum.
Aneshensel, C. S. and G. J. Huba
1983 Depression, alcohol use, and smoking over one year: A
four-wave
longitudinal
causal model. Journal
of
Abnormal Psychology 92:134-150.
Asher, S. R. and P. D. Renshaw
1981 Children without friends: social knowledge and social skill training. In S. R.
Asher and J. M. Gottman (eds.),The Development
of
Children's Friendships. New
York:
Cambridge University Press.
Ashton, H. and R. Stepney
1982 Smoking: Psychology
and
Pharmacology. New
York:
Tavistock.
Bachman, J. G.
1982 Family relationships and self-esteem. In M. Rosenberg and H. B. Kaplan (eds.),
Social Psychology and the
Self
Concept. Arlington Heights, Illinois: Harlan
Davidson.
Bachman, J. G., L. D. Johnston and P. M. O'Malley
1981 Monitoring the Future: Questionnaire Responses from the Nation's High School
Seniors 1980.Ann Arbor,Michigan: Institute for Social Research.
Bandura, A.
1977 Social Learning
Theory.
EnglewoodCliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.
Barnes, G. M., M. P. Farrell and A. Cairns
1986 Parental socialization factors and adolescent drinking behaviors. Journal
of
Marriage
and
the Family 48:27-36.
308 JOURNAL OF DRUG ISSUES
A
MULTISTAGE
SOCIAL LEARNING MODEL
Baumrind, D.
1985 Familial antecedants of adolescent drug use: A developmental perspective. In
C. L. Jones and R. J. Battjes (eds.), Etiology
of
Drug Abuse: Implications for
Prevention. Rockville, Maryland: National Institute on Drug Abuse.
Beck, A. T. and A. J. Rush
1978 Cognitive approaches to depression and suicide. In G. Serban (ed.), Cognitive
Defects in the Development
of
Mental Illness. New York: Brunner-Mazel.
Bowlby,
J.
1980 Attachment
and
Loss:
Vol.
3. Loss: Sadness
and
Depression. New
York:
Basic.
Brittain, C. V.
1963 Adolescent choices
and
parent-peer cross-pressures. American Sociological
Review 28:385-391.
Brook, J. S., I. F. Lukoff and M. Whiteman
1977 Peer, family and personality domains as related to adolescent's drug behavior.
Psychological Reports, 41:1095-1102.
Burkett, S. R. and M. White
1984 Hellfire and delinquency: Another look. Journal
of
the Scientific
Study
of
Religion 13:455-462.
Cernkovich, S. A.
1978a Evaluating two models of delinquency causation: Structural
and
control theory.
Criminology 16:355-362.
Cernkovich, S. A.
1978b Value orientations and delinquency involvement. Criminology 15:443-448.
Chartier, G. M.
and
D. J. Ranieri
1984 Adolescent depression: Concepts, treatments,
and
prevention. In P. Karoly and
J. J. Steffan (eds.), Adolescent Behavior Disorders: Foundations
and
Contempo-
rary Concerns. Lexington, Massachusetts: Lexington Books.
Clayton, R. R.
1984 The delinquency and drug use relationship among adolescents: A critical review.
In D. J. Lettieri and J. P. Ludford (eds.), Drug Abuse
and
the American
Adolescent. Washington, D.C.: National Institute on Drug Abuse.
Cloward, R.
and
L. Ohlin
1960 Delinquency
and
Opportunity. New York:Free Press, 1960.
Cohen, A. K
1955 Delinquent Boys: The Subculture
of
the Gang. New York:Free Press.
Cohen,J. M.
1977 Sources of peer group homogeneity. Sociology
of
Education, 50:227-241.
Conger, R. D.
1976 Social control and social learning models of delinquent behavior. Criminology,
14:17-40.
Conger, R. D.
1980 Juvenile delinquency: Behavior
restraint
or behavior facilitation? In T. Hirschi
and M. Gottfredson (eds.), Understanding Crime: Current Theory
and
Research.
Beverly Hills: Sage.
Conger, R. D.
1981 The assessment of dysfunctional family systems. In B. Lahey and A. Kazdin
(eds.),Advances in Child Clinical Psychology
(Vol.
3). New York: Plenum.
Conger,R. D., J. A. McCarty, R. KYoung, B. B. Lahey and J. P. Kropp
1984 Perception of child, child-rearing values, and emotional distress as mediating
links between environmental stressors and observed maternal behavior. Child
Development, 55:2234-2247.
Coopersmith, S.
1967 The Antecedants
of
Self
Esteem. San Francisco:
W.
H. Freeman.
Summer
1988 309
SIMONS
Council on Scientific Affairs
1982 Drug abuse related to prescribing practices. Journal
of
the American Medical
Association, 247:864-866.
Day,N.
and
K Leonard
1985 Alcohol, drug use and psychopathology in the general population. In A. I.
Alterman (ed.), Substance Abuse
and
Psychopathology. New York: Plenum.
Dishion, T.J., G. R. Patterson and J. B. Reid
1985 Parenting practices in the etiology
of
child drug use: Implications for treatment
and
prevention. Paper presented to
the
NIDA Technical Review Committee on
Adolescent Drug Abuse: Analysis of Treatment and Research, Washington, D.C.
Dishion, T.J., M. Stouthamer-Loeber and G. R. Patterson
1984 The monitoring construct. Eugene, Oregon: Oregan Social Learning Center.
Dodge, K. A.
1983 Behavioral antecedents of peer social status. Child Development 54:1386-1399.
Elder, G.
1974 Children of the Great Depression. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Elder, G. H., A. Caspi and G. Downey
1983 Problem behavior in family relationships: Amultigenerational analysis. In A.
Sorensen, F.Weinert, and L. Sherrod (eds.),
Human
Development: Interdisciplin-
ary Perspectives. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates:
Elifson, K. W.,D.
W.
Peterson and C. K. Hedaway
1983 Religiosity and delinquency. Criminology 21:515-527.
Elliott, D. S. and A. R. Ageton
1976 Subcultural Delinquency
and
Drug Use. Boulder, Colorado: Behavioral Research
Institute.
Elliott, D. S., D. Huizinga and S. S. Ageton
1985 Explaining Delinquency
and
Drug Use. Beverly Hills: Sage.
Feather, N. T.
1975 Values in Education
and
Society. New York:Free Press.
Fishburne, P., H. Abelson and I. Cisin
1980 The National Survey on Drug Use: Main Findings, 1979. Washington, D.C.: U.S.
Govt. Print. Off.
Fleming, J.
P.,
S. G. Kellam and C. H. Brown
1982 Early predictors of age at first use of alcohol, marijuana and cigarettes. Drug
and Alcohol Dependence 9:285-303.
Ginsberg, 1.J. and J. R. Greenley
1978 Competing theories of marijuana use: A longitudinal study. Journal
of
Health
and
Social Behavior, 19:22-34.
Greenberg, M. T.,J. M. Siegel and C. J. Leitch
1983 The nature and importance of attachment relationships to parents and peers
during adolescence. Journal
of
Youth
and
Adolescence 12:373-385.
Hartup,
W.
W.
1983 Peer relations. In P. H. Mussen (ed.),Handbook
of
Child Psychology.
Vol.
Iv. New
York:
Wiley.
Henningfleld, J. E., S. E. Lukas and G. E. Bigelow
1986 Human Studies of Drugs as Reinforcers. In S. R. Goldberg and 1.P. Stolerman
(eds.),Behavioral Analysis
of
Drug Dependence. New York:Academic.
Hirschi, T.
1969 Causes of Delinquency. Berkeley, California: Univ. of California Press.
Hodges, K K. and L. J. Siegel
1985 Depression in Children and Adolescents. In E. E. Beckham and
W.
R. Leber
. (eds.), Handbook
of
Depression: Treatment, Assessment,
and
Research. Home-
wood,Illinois: Dorsey.
310 JOURNAL OF DRUG ISSUES
A MULTISTAGE SOCIAL LEARNING MODEL
Hoffman, M. L.
1983 Empathy, guilt
and
social cognition. In W. F. Overton (ed.), The Relationship
Between Social and Cognitive Development. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Huba, G. J., J. A. Wingard
and
P. M. Bentler
1979 Beginning adolescent drug use
and
peer
and
adult
interaction. Journal
of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 47:265-276.
Huba, G. J., J. A. Wingard
and
P. M. Bentler
1980 Longitudinal analysis of
the
role of peer support,
adult
models
and
peer
subcultures in beginning adolescent substance use. Multivariate Behavioral
Research. 15:259-280.
Huesmann, L. R., L. D. Eron, M. N. Lefkowitz
and
L. 0. Wlader
1983 The Stability of Aggression Over Time and Generations. Paper presented at
the
meeting of
the
Society for Research in Child Development, Detroit, Michigan.
Jensen, G. F.
and
M. L. Erickson
1979 The religious factor
and
delinquency: Another look at the hellfire hypothesis. In
R. Wutknow (ed.), The Religious Dimension. New York: Academic.
Jessor, R.
and
S. L. Jessor
1977 Problem Behavior and Psychosocial Development-A Longitudinal Study
of
Youth. New York: Academic.
Johanson, C. E.
and
E. H.
Uhlenhuth
1980 Drug preference
and
mood in humans: d-amphetamine. Psychopharmacology
71:275-279.
Johnston, L. D., P. M. O'Malley
and
L. K. Eveland
1978 Drugs
and
delinquency: Asearch for causal connections. In D. B. Kandel (ed.),
Longitudinal Research on Drug Use: Empirical Findings and Methodological
Issues. Washington, D.C.: Hemisphere.
Kandel, D. B.
1975 Stages in adolescent drug use. Science, 190:912-914.
Kandel, D. B.
1976 Adolescent involvement in illicit
drug
use: A multiple classification analysis.
Social
Fbrces
55:438-458.
Kandel, D. B.
1978 Homophily, selection
and
socialization in adolescent friendships. American
Journal of Sociology, 84:427-436.
Kandel, D. B.
1983 Commonalities in drug use: A sociological perspective. In P. K. Levison, D. R.
Gerstein,
and
D. R. Maloff (eds.), Commonalities in Substance Abuse and
Habitual Behavior. Lexington, Massachusetts: Lexington Books.
Kandel, D., R. Kessler
and
R. Margulies
1978 Antecedents of adolescent initiation into stages of
drug
use: A developmental
analysis. Journal
of
Youth and Adolescence, 7:13-40.
Kandel, D. B.
and
K. Yamaguchi
1985 Developmental
patterns
of
the
use of legal, illegal,
and
medically prescribed
psychotropic drugs from adolescence to young adulthood. In C. L. Jones
and
R.J.
Battjes (eds.), Etiology of Drug Abuse: Implications for Prevention. Rockville,
Maryland: National
Institute
on Drug Abuse.
Kandel, D. B.
and
K. Yamaguchi
1987 Job mobility
and
drug
use: An event history analysis. American Journal
of
Sociology 92:836-878.
Kaplan, H. B.
1975 Increases in self-rejection as an antecedent of deviant response. Journal
of
Youth
and Adolescence 4:281-292.
SUmmer
1988 311
SIMONS
Kaplan, H. B.
1976 Self-attitudes
and
deviant response. Social Forces54:788-801.
Kaplan, H. B.
1977 Increase in self-rejection
and
continuing/discontinued deviant response. Journal
of
Youth
and
Adolescence 6:77-87.
Kaplan, H. B.
1980 Deviant Behavior in Defense
of
Self. New York: Academic.
Kaplan, H. B.
1982 Self-attitudes
and
deviant behavior: New directions for theory
and
research.
Youth and Society 14:185-211.
Kaplan, H. B., R. J. Johnson and C. A. Bailey
1986 Self-rejection
and
the
explanation of deviance: Specification of
the
structure
among
latent
constructs. American Journal
of
Sociology 92:384-411.
Kaplan, H. B.,S. S.
Martin
and
C. Robbins
1984 Pathways to adolescent
drug
use: Self-derogation, peer influence, weakening of
social controls, and early substance use. Journal
of
Health
and
Social Behavior
25:270-288.
Kellam, S. G.
and
H. Brown
1982 Social Adaptational
and
Psychological Antecedants
of
Adolescent Psycho-
pathology
71m
Years Later. Baltimore:
Johns
Hopkins University.
Kessler, R. C., R. H. Price
and
C. B. Wortman
1985 Social factors in psychopathology: Stress, social support,
and
coping processes.
Annual
Review
of
Psychology 36:531-572.
Lahey, B. B., R. D. Conger, B. M. Atkeson
and
F.A. Treifer
1984
Parenting
behavior
and
emotional
status
of physically abusive mothers. Journal
of Consulting
and
Clinical Psychology, 52:1062-1072.
Lawson, G., J. S. Peterson
and
A. Lawson
1983 Alcoholism
and
the Family. Rockville, Maryland: Aspen Publications.
Lettieri, D. J., M. Sayers
and
J.
W.
Pearson, eds.
1980 Theories on Drug Abuse: Selected Contemporary Perspectives. Rockville, Mary-
land: National
Institute
on Drug Abuse.
Maccoby,E.
and
J.
Martin
1983 Socialization in
the
context of
the
family: Parent-child interaction. In P.Mussen
(ed.), Handbook
of
Child Psychology. New York:
John
Wlley &Sons.
MacLennan, A. J.
and
S. F. Maier
1983 Coping and
the
stress-induced potentiation of
stimulant
stereotypy in
the
rat.
Science, 219:1091-1093.
McCarthy, J. D. and D. R. Hoge
1984 The dynamics of self-esteem
and
delinquency. American Journal
of
Sociology
90:396-410.
McKennel, A. C.
and
R. KThomas
1967 Adults'
and
Adolescents' Smoking Habits
and
Attitudes. London, British Minis-
try
of Health.
Marlatt, G. A.
1979 Alcohol use
and
problem drinking: Acognitive-behavioral analysis. In P. C.
Kendall
and
S. D. Hollon (eds.), Cognitive Behavioral Interventions: Theory,
Research,
and
Procedure. New York: Academic.
Martin, W.R.
1983 Thoughts about
the
relevance of neurochemistry, neurophysiology
and
neuro-
psychoopharmacology to
the
social problems of drug abuse. Drug
and
Alcohol
Dependence 11:9-10.
Matza, D.
1964 Delinquency
and
Drift. New York:
Wiley.
312 JOURNAL OF DRUG ISSUES
A
MULTISTAGE
SOCIAL LEARNING MODEL
Miller,J. D.
1981 Drug abuse
and
the American Adolescent. National Institute on Drug Abuse
Research Monograph Series No. 38. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Print-
ing
Office.
Mirin, S. M.
1984 Substance Abuse
and
Psychopathology. Washington, D.C.:American Psychiatric
Press.
NettIer, G.
1984 Explaining Crime. 3rd ed. New
York:
McGraw-Hill.
Nicholi, A. M., Jr.
1983 The nontherapeutic use of psychoactive drugs. New England Journal
of
Medi-
cine, 308:925-983.
O'Malley, P.M. and J. G. Bachman
1979 Self-esteem and education: Sex and cohort comparisons Among High School
Seniors. Journal
of
Personality
and
Social Psychology 37:1153-1159.
Orcutt, J.
1978 Normative definitions ofintoxicated states: Atest ofseveral sociologicaltheories.
Social Problems, 4:385-396.
Parry, M. A. and M. J. FUrukawa
1980 Modeling methods. In F. H. Kanfer and A. P. Goldstein (eds.), Helping People
Change. 2nd ed. New York:Pergamon.
Patterson, G. R.
1982 Coercive Family Process. Eugene, Oregon: Castalia.
Patterson, G. R.
1983 Stress: A change agent for family process. In N. Garmezy and M. Rutter (eds.),
Stress, Coping,
and
Development in Children. New
York:
McGraw-Hill.
Patterson, G. R.
1986 Performance models for antisocial boys. American Psychologist 41:432-444.
Patterson, G. R. and T. J. Dishion
1985 Contribution to families and peers to delinquency. Criminology 23:63-79.
Patterson, G. R. and M. Stouthammer-Loeber
1984 The correlation offamily management practices and delinquency. ChildDevelop-
ment 55:1320-1307.
Pearlin, L. I. and C. S. Schooler
1978 The structure of coping. Journal
of
Health
and
Social Behavior 19:2-21.
Penning, M. and G. E. Barnes
1982 Adolescent marijuana use: A
review.
International Journal
of
the Addictions
17:749-791.
Pentz, M. A.
1985 Social competence and self-efficacyas determinants of substance use in adoles-
cence. In S. Siffman and T. A. Wills (eds.),Coping
and
Substance Use. NewYork:
Academic.
Peterson, C. and M. E. P. Seligman
1985 The learned helplessness model of depression: Current status of theory and
research. In S. E. Beckham and W. R. Leber (eds.), Handbook
of
Depression:
Treatment, Assessment,
and
Research. Homewood, Illinois: Dorsey.
Putallaz, M. and J. M. Gottman
1981 Social skills and group acceptance. In S. R. Asher and J. M. Gottman (eds.),
7'1u1
Development
of
Children's Friendships. New York:Cambridge University Press.
Reilly,D. M.
1979 Family factors in the etiology and treatment of youthful drug abuse. Family
Therapy 11:149-171.
Summer
1988 818
SIMONS
Robins, L. N. and G. E. Murphy
1967 Drug use in a normal population of young negro men. American Journal of
Health. 57:1580-1596.
Robins, L. N. and T. R. Przybeck
1985 Age of onset of drug use as a factor in drug and other disorders. In C. L. Jones
and R. J. Battjes (eds.), Etiology
of
Drug Abuse: Implications for Prevention.
Rockville,Maryland: National Institute of Drug Abuse.
Rohner, R. P.
1986 The Warmth Dimension: Foundations
of
Parental Acceptance-Rejection Theory.
Beverly Hills, California: Sage.
Rokeach, M.
1968 Beliefs, Attitudes
and
Values. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Rokeach, M.
1973 The Nature
of
Human
Values. New
York:
Free.
Rosenberg, M.
1965 Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Rouse, B. A., P. F. Walker and J. A. Ewing
1973 Adolescent stress levels, coping activities, and father's drinking behavior. Pro-
ceedings
of
the 81st
Annual
Convention of the American Psychological Associa-
tion 8:683-684.
Sebald, H.
1986 Adolescents' shifting orientation toward parents and peers: A curvilinear trend
over recent decades. Journal
of
Marriage
and
the Family 48:5-14.
Seligman, M. E. P., L.
Y.
Abramson, A. Semmel and C. Von Baeyer
1979 Depressive attributional style. Journal
of
Abnormal Psychology 88:242-248.
Shiffman, S. and T. A. Wills,eds.
1985 Coping
and
Substance Use. NewYork: Academic.
Simons, R. L. and P. I. Murphy
1985 Sex differences in the causes of adolescent suicide ideation. Journal
of
Youth
and
Adolescence 14:423-434.
Snyder, J., T. J. Dishion and G. R. Patterson
1986 Determinants and consequences of associating with deviant peers during pre-
adolescence and adolescence. Journal
of
Early Adolescence 6:29-43.
Stone, A. A., S. Lennox and J. M. Neale
1985 Daily coping and alcohol use in a sample of community adults. In S. Shiffman
and T. A. Wills (eds.), Coping
and
Substance Use. New
York:
Academic.
Timmer, S. G., J. Veroffand M. E. Colten
1985 Lifestress, helplessness, and the use of alcohol and drugs to cope:An analysis of
national survey data. In S. Shiffman and T. A. Wills (eds.)., Coping
and
Substance Use. New York:Academic.
Uhlenhuth, E. H., C. E. Johanson, S. Kilgore and S. C. Kosaba
1981 Drug preferences and mood in humans: Preference for d-amphetamine and
subject characteristics. Psychopharmacology 74:191-194.
VoId,
G. B. and T. J. Bernard
1986 Theoretical Criminology. 3rd ed. New
York:
Oxford.
Wegscheider, S.
1981 Another Chance: Hope
and
Health for the Alcoholic Family. Palo Alto: Science
and Behavior.
Wells,L. E. and J. H. Rankin
1983 Self-concept as a mediating factor in delinquency. Social Psychology Quarterly
46:11-22.
314
JOURNAL OF DRUG ISSUES
A
MULTISTAGE
SOCIAL LEARNING MODEL
Wilks, J.
1986 The relative importance of parents and friends in adolescent decision making.
Journal
of
Youth
and
Adolescence 15:323-335.
Wills,T.A.
1985 Stress, coping, and tobacco and alcohol use in early adolescence. In S. Shiffman
and T. A. Wills (eds.), Coping
and
Substance Use. NewYork:Academic.
Wills,T. A. and S. Shiffman
1985 Coping and substance use: A conceptual framework. In S. Shiffman and T. A.
Wl1ls
(eds.),Coping
and
Substance Use. NewYork:Academic.
Wrubel,J., P.Benner and R. S. Lazarus
1981 Social competence from the perspective of stress and coping. In J. D. Wine and
M. D. Smye (eds.), Social Competence. New York: Guilford.
Yamaguchi, K and D. B. Kandel
1985 On the resolution of role incompatibility: A life event history analysis of family
roles and marijuana use. American Journal
of
Sociology 90:1284-1325.
Yost,
J. K and R. A. Mines
1985 Stress and alcoholism. In T. E. Bratter and G. G. Forrest (eds.), Alcoholism
and
Substance Abuse: Strategies for Clinical Intervention. New York:Free.
Young,
A. M. and S. Herling
1986 Drugs as reinforcers: Studies in laboratory animals. In S. R. Goldberg and I. P.
Stolerman (eds.),Behavioral Analysis
of
Drug Dependence. New York:Academic.
Sununer1988
3Ui

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT