Momentum, Reversals, and Fund Manager Overconfidence

Published date01 August 2016
AuthorXuemin (Sterling) Yan,Biljana N. Adebambo
Date01 August 2016
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/fima.12128
Momentum, Reversals, and Fund
Manager Overconfidence
Biljana N. Adebambo and Xuemin (Sterling) Yan
This paper examines the role of investor overconfidence and self-attribution bias in explaining
the momentum effect. We develop a novel measure of overconfidence based on characteristics
and trading patterns of US equity mutual fund managers. Stocks held by more overconfident
managers experience greater momentum profits and stronger return reversals than stocksheld by
less overconfident managers. The difference in momentum profits is not compensation for risk nor
is it attributable to stock characteristicsthat influence momentum. Our results are consistent with
Daniel, Hirshleifer, and Subrahmanyam (1998) who argue that momentum results from delayed
overreaction caused by overconfidence and biased self-attribution.
In this paper, weexamine whether investor overconfidence, combined with self-attribution bias,
contributes to the momentum effect of Jegadeesh and Titman(1993). The momentum effect, or the
tendency of recent winners to outperform recent losers over the subsequent 3 to 12 months, is the
most prominent anomaly unexplained by the Fama-French three-factor model (Fama and French,
1996). Moreover, the momentum effect has been documented around the world (Rouwenhorst,
1998; Griffin, Ji, and Martin, 2003) across many asset classes (Asness, Moskowitz, and Pedersen,
2013) and remains significant after its initial discovery (Jegadeesh and Titman, 2001).
Although several theoretical and empirical papers offerrational explanations for the momentum
effect, the literature has primarily focused on behavioral explanations due to the magnitude
of momentum profits (Chui, Titman, and Wei, 2010).1For instance, Barberis, Shleifer, and
Vishny (1998) and Hong and Stein (1999) propose models in which investors’ conservatism and
the slow diffusion of news cause initial under-reaction to information and lead to momentum.
Alternatively, Daniel et al. (1998) develop a model in which investor overconfidence and biased
self-attribution generate delayed overreaction to information and result in momentum. In this
paper, we empirically examine the Daniel et al. (1998) explanation for the momentum effect.
This paper is written in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a Doctorate of Philosophy from the University of
Missouri. We thank Marc Lipson (Editor), an anonymous referee, Debarati Bhattacharya, Stephen Ferris, Grace Hao,
John Howe, Dong Lou, Ulrike Malmendier, Shawn Ni, Shastri Sandy, and seminar participants at the University of
Memphis, University of Missouri, University of San Diego, University of Wyoming, Miami University, the 2011 FMA
Annual Meeting, the 2011 FMA Doctoral Consortium, and the 2012 AFA Annual Meeting for helpful comments. Xuemin
(Sterling) Yanacknowledges the support of Chinese National Science Foundation (Grant #71432008).All errors are our
own.
Biljana N. Adebambo is an Assistant Professor at the School of Business at the University of San Diego in San Diego,
CA. Xuemin (Sterling) Yanis a Professor in the Robert J. Trulaske, Sr. College of Business at the University of Missouri
in Columbia, MO.
1Rational momentum models include Berk, Green, and Naik (1999), Johnson (2002), and Sagi and Seasholes (2007).
As discussed in Chui et al. (2010), the biggest challenge for these models is to explain the magnitude of momentum
profits without assuming extreme levels of risk aversion. Among empirical papers, Conrad and Kaul (1998) and Chordia
and Shivakumar (2002) provideevidence that momentum is related to expected returns. However, Jegadeesh and Titman
(2001), Grundy and Martin (2001), and Cooper, Gutierrez, and Hameed (2004) challenge the robustness of their findings.
Financial Management Fall 2016 pages 609 – 639
610 Financial Management rFall 2016
Daniel et al. (1998) study an informed, but overconfident investor whooverreacts to his private
signal. If subsequent public information confirms this signal, it triggers further overreaction due
to self-attribution bias resulting in stock price momentum. In the long run, as more information
becomes available,prices gradually move to fundamentals reversing the initial overreaction. Thus,
if overconfidence drives the momentum effect, we expect both short run momentum and long
run reversal to be stronger for stocks predominantly owned by overconfident investors.
Several recent empirical papers offer evidence consistent with the hypothesis that overconfi-
dence impacts momentum. Cooper, Gutierrez, and Hameed (2004) find that momentum profits
exist only in periods following prolonged market gains. Although aggregate overconfidence
should be greater following market gains, market state by itself is not a measure of overconfi-
dence.2Chui et al. (2010) confirm that momentum prof its are higher in countries with stronger
individualism. While the authors argue that individualism is correlated with overconfidence and
self-attribution bias, they acknowledge that “. . . it does not directly measure the behavioral biases
suggested in the momentum literature.” (p. 362). In this paper, we focus on the overconfidence
of mutual fund managers and use it as a conditioning variable to provide evidencesuppor ting the
Daniel et al. (1998) hypothesis.3
To develop the overconfidence measure, we use a comprehensive sample of mutual fund
managers. In developing the measure, we focus on mutual fund managers’ overconfidence for
four reasons. First, the psychology literature suggests that overconfidence should be stronger
among professional investors (Heath and Tversky, 1991; Griffin and Tversky,1992). In addition,
Daniel et al. (1998) model overconfidence as an investor’s overestimationof the precision of their
private information and professional investors, such as mutual fund managers, are more likelyto
possess private information. Moreover, mutual funds hold a large and growing fraction of the US
stock market. According to the Investment Company Institute Fact Book (2015), mutual funds
held 24% of the US stock market at the end of 2014. Finally, detailed characteristics and holdings
data are readily available for mutual funds and their managers.
However, overconfidence is not directly observable. To overcome this challenge, we construct
an overconfidence index by combining six overconfidence and self-attribution bias proxies sug-
gested in the prior literature. Specifically, our overconfidence index includes manager’s gender,
manager’s tenure, portfolio turnover, portfolio concentration, prior performance, and idiosyn-
cratic risk. The index approach has three distinct advantages. It is parsimonious, it reduces the
noise associated with individual proxies, and, most importantly, it allows us to capture multiple
dimensions of overconfidence and the self-attribution bias.
To provide robustness to the analysis, we construct two versions of the overconfidence index.
The first version of the overconfidence index equally weights each of the six proxies. The second
version of the index is the first principal component of the six proxies. Since overconfidence is a
manager-level characteristic, while momentum is a stock-level anomaly, for each version of the
index, we compute the stock-level overconfidence index as the weighted average overconfidence
index of all fund managers who hold the stock, using the manager’s holdings of the stock as a
weight. We use the two resulting stock-level overconfidence indexes as conditioning variables in
the analysis.
2In a related paper, Asem and Tian (2010) find that momentum profits are stronger when markets continue in the same
state than when they transition to a different state, thereby supporting Daniel et al. (1998) and rejecting Hong and Stein
(1999), as well as Sagi and Seasholes (2007). However, once again, the continuation of the market state is not a direct
measure of overconfidence.
3For ease of exposition, we use the term “overconfidence” to refer to “dynamic overconfidence due to self-attribution
bias.” Our overconfidence measure captures both overconfidence and the self-attribution bias.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT