Is Unstructured Socializing a Dynamic Process? An Exploratory Analysis Using a Semiparametric Group-Based Modeling Approach

AuthorWesley G. Jennings,George E. Higgins
Published date01 December 2010
Date01 December 2010
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1177/0734016810377351
Subject MatterArticles
Is Unstructured Socializing a
Dynamic Process? An
Exploratory Analysis Using a
Semiparametric Group-Based
Modeling Approach
George E. Higgins
1
, and Wesley G. Jennings
2
Abstract
Relying on the theory of Osgood et al. of unstructured socializing (US) and using longitudinal data
from the Gang Resistance Education and Training (G.R.E.A.T.) program, this study examines
whether US is a dynamic process and how the changes in US may relate to changes in
delinquency during adolescence for groups, who demonstrate different levels of involvement and
frequency of offending over time. Results from semiparametric group-based models indicate that
individuals tend to follow three distinct trajectories of US and four trajectories of delinquency.
Furthermore, additional results from multinomial regressions suggest that key covariates can signif-
icantly distinguish group membership and that the covariates that distinguish US trajectories distin-
guish the delinquency trajectories. Finally, the results demonstrate that there is a considerable
degree of overlap across trajectory groups. That is, those that follow the high-rate US trajectory
also tend to follow the high-rate delinquency trajectory. Study limitations and possible theoretical
implications are also discussed.
Keywords
unstructured socializing, opportunity, delinquency, crime, trajectories
Introduction
Few in criminology would argue that delinquent peer associations are not an important correlate
of delinquency (Akers, 1985; 1998; Haynie, 2001; Warr, 2002). Considering this relatively robust
association, a number of theoretical perspectives have commented on why peers play such an impor-
tant role in delinquency (e.g., see Agnew, 1992, 2006; Akers, 1998; Hirschi, 1969). Although these
1
Department of Justice Administration, College of Arts and Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, USA
2
University of South Florida, Tampa FL
Corresponding Author:
George E. Higgins, Department of Justice Administration, College of Arts and Sciences, University of Louisville, 2301 South
3rd Street, 208 Brigman Hall, Louisville, KY 40292, USA
Email: gehigg01@gwise.louisville.edu
Criminal Justice Review
35(4) 514-532
ª2010 Georgia State University
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0734016810377351
http://cjr.sagepub.com
514
classic general theories of crime differ in their assumptions and explanations on how and why peers
matter and at what stages peers may matter more for contributing to delinquency, they nevertheless
agree that peers are significant.
Expanding beyond these classic general theories of crime, Osgood, Wilson, Bachman, O’Malley,
and Johnson (1996) have since added to the theoretical discussion regarding the role of peers in
delinquency. More specifically, the theory of Osgood et al. is conceptually a modification of
the basic tenets of routine activities theory recognizing the importance of its three key elements:
(a) a motivated offender (with the motivation coming from the situation itself and not internal to the
individual); (b) a suitable target (which is part of the situation and is a person or object; the suitabil-
ity of the situation is determined by the possibility of the reward); and (c) a lack of capable guardian-
ship (peers or capable adults that may not be bonded with the individual and, thus, performing social
control to stop delinquent acts). Furthermore, central to the theory of Osgood et al. is unstructured
time spent with peers. For Osgood et al., delinquency is possible when the individual spends a sub-
stantial amount of time with peers outside of the purview of capable guardians. It is the expectation
that when an individual engages in delinquency, peers provide the rewards and status for the act.
Following from this initial reinforcement process, the rewards and the increase in status are believed
to supply the qualities necessary for the continuation of delinquency. Thus, the theory of Osgood
et al. is one that accounts for the initiation and continuation of delinquent behavior.
Although this theory has been in existence for some time now in the criminological literature, the
theory has not been extensively explored, but those that have explored the theory have demonstrated
support for the relationship between unstructured socializing (US) and delinquency (Haynie &
Osgood, 2005; Osgood & Anderson, 2004; Osgood et al., 1996). However, acknowledging that these
tests revealed support for the theory of Osgood et al., they did not provide an examination of (a) the
changes that may take place with US over time; (b) the predictors of the changes that may take place;
and (c) the relationship between the changes that may take place in US and delinquency.
Attending to the gap in this line of literature as outlined above, the purpose of the current study is
threefold. First, we investigate if and to what extent US is in fact a dynamic process that may follow
trajectories. Second, guided by prior literature (Osgood & Anderson, 2004) and operating under the
assumption that there are distinct trajectories of US, we examine whether key covariates are able to
distinguish trajectories of US and delinquency. Third, we assess the possible link/overlap between
the trajectories of US and the trajectories of delinquency.
US
As mentioned previously, Osgood et al. (1996) derived their version of US from the basic tenets
of routine activities theory (see Cohen & Felson, 1979; Felson, 1998). They suggest that delinquency
is possible when an individual is away from his or her home or family when a situation provides
ample motivation. Consequently, when adolescents are in the company of their peers without super-
vision from authority figures (i.e., adults) the opportunity for delinquency increases. This is not to
argue that US is a necessary condition for delinquent behavior, but rather that it makes delinquency
more likely because no authority figure is present to help the adolescent avoid the temptation. Thus,
it is in this particular situation where there is the lack of an authority figure, when an individual is
more likely to succumb to an opportunity of delinquency. This does not mean that the lack of an
authority figure and US generate the motivation for delinquency. However, Osgood et al. argue that
the motivation for delinquency comes from the act being possible and rewarding.
Peers make delinquency appealing because they provide an opportunity to reward delinquency as
well as the means to make delinquency easier to perform, thereby making the behavior possible and
rewarding. Peers are consistently known for making delinquency easier to engage in because they
may assist in the performance of delinquency. That is, peers do not have to provide tangible rewards
Higgins and Jennings 515
515

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT