Examining the interpersonal process and consequence of leader–member exchange comparison: The role of procedural justice climate

AuthorCatherine K. Lam,Jun Gu,Herman H. M. Tse,Xiao Song Lin
Published date01 October 2018
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1002/job.2268
Date01 October 2018
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Examining the interpersonal process and consequence of
leadermember exchange comparison: The role of procedural
justice climate
Herman H. M. Tse
1
|Catherine K. Lam
2
|Jun Gu
1
|Xiao Song Lin
3
1
Department of Management, Monash
University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
2
Department of Management, City University
of Hong Kong, Kowloon, Hong Kong
3
School of Management, Xiamen University,
Xiamen, Fujian, China
Correspondence
Corresponding Xiao Song Lin, School of
Management, Xiamen University, Xiamen,
Fujian, China.
Email: xiaosong.lin@xmu.edu.cn
Summary
Individuals are always sensitive to their relative standing in interpersonal comparison processes of
leadermember exchanges (LMXs) in teams. Little research, however, has investigated whether
coworkers with a higher LMX influence the emotional and behavioral reactions of individuals with
a lower LMX in different dyads. Drawing on social comparison theory and the symbolic model of
procedural justice (PJ) climate, we conducted 2 independent studiesan experimental study
focusing on the selfperceived upward LMX comparison (i.e., an individual perceives that a
coworker's LMX is higher than the LMX that he or she has with the supervisor; N= 203; Study
1: American working adults) and a field survey study focusing on the otherperceived downward
LMX comparison (i.e., a coworker perceives that his or her own LMX is higher than the LMX that
the individual has with the supervisor; N= 177; Study 2: Chinese software engineers). Results
from these studies consistently revealed that a coworker's higher LMX elicits an individual'shos-
tile emotions when the PJ climate is low but that this relationship is buffered when the PJ climate
is high. Results of both studies also showed that the coworker's higher LMX arouses the individ-
ual to direct harmful behavior toward that coworker (via the individual's feelings of hostility)
when the PJ climate is low but not when it is high.
KEYWORDS
coworker dyads,interpersonal hostility and harming,leadermember exchange (LMX) comparison,
procedural justice climate
1|INTRODUCTION
A key premise of leadermember exchange (LMX) theory is that
leaders tend to develop different relationships with individuals in a
team (Dansereau, Graen, & Haga, 1975). Because the differentiation
determines the amount of workrelated resources, benefits, and
support obtained from a supervisor, individuals are always sensitive
to their relative LMX (RLMX) standing in a team (Vidyarthi, Liden,
Anand, Erdogan, & Ghosh, 2010). During their daily interactions in
the workplace, individuals often learn, reflect, observe, and compare
their own LMX with that of their coworkers in order to understand
their standing in the team (Hu & Liden, 2013). Previous research has
tended to view an LMX comparison as an individual process of RLMX
that occurs when an individual evaluates his or her own actual level of
LMX relative to the average LMX of allother coworkers as a whole
(e.g., Henderson, Wayne, Shore, Bormmer, & Tetrick, 2008), and it
has also examined the impact of RLMX on psychological contract
fulfilment (Henderson et al., 2008), inrole and extrarole behaviors
(Hu & Liden, 2013), and task performance (Vidyarthi et al., 2010).
Although the findings of these studies are insightful, our under-
standing of the LMX comparison process remains incomplete owing to
two fundamental issues. First, LMX comparison is often studied as an
individual phenomenon, captured either by an objective measure of a
relative difference between coworkers' LMXs (i.e., RLMX; Henderson
et al., 2008; Tse, Ashkanasy, & Dasborough, 2012) or by a perceptual
measure of individuals' own LMX compared with the LMXs of
coworkers (i.e., LMX social comparison [LMXSC]; Vidyarthi et al.,
2010). This study approach overlooks the fact that the LMX comparison
process is dyadic and peer nested in nature. Second, the existing
research has generally focused on understanding the positive
consequences (i.e., both individual and team performancerelated
outcomes) when an individual interacts with lower LMX coworkers
and perceives that his or her LMX is better than other coworkers' LMXs
(Tse, Lam, Lawrence, & Huang, 2013; Vidyarthi et al., 2010). This line of
Received: 14 October 2016 Revised: 16 December 2017 Accepted: 6 January 2018
DOI: 10.1002/job.2268
922 Copyright © 2018 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J Organ Behav. 2018;39:922940.wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/job
research has neglected the possibility that an individual may have to
face a higher LMX coworker, and such an unfavorable comparison can
create negative interpersonal implications (Tse et al., 2013). This is an
important omission in the literature because, in reality, the individual
is often required to work with another coworker who has a higher
LMX, and such a situation may emanate from the facts that (a) he or
she perceives a coworker's LMX to be higher than he or she has with
the supervisor (i.e., an individual has a upward LMX comparison with
the coworkeran individual's upward LMXSC) or (b) he or she is
perceived to be an individual with a lower LMX compared with the
coworker (i.e., a coworker has a downward LMX comparison with the
individuala coworker's downward LMXSC). Althoughthese two types
of comparison are independently evaluated by the individual and the
coworker, both forms of LMX comparison inform an individual about
another coworker's higher relative standing and also highlight his or
her inferior status in a team, thereby causing the individual to react
negatively toward the coworker. Such a reaction occurs because
information comparing LMXs among coworkers infers hierarchical
status differences, which can often be learned and observed in
coworkers' daily interactions within a shared team environment (Sias,
1996; Tse et al., 2012, 2013). Therefore, with the use of a dyadic
approach, the core contributionof the present research is to understand
whether a coworker's higher LMX (by directly manipulating the
individual's upward LMXSC in Study 1 and by indirectly inferring an
unfavorable comparison outcome by assessing the coworker's
downward LMXSC in Study 2) may induce the individual's negative
emotion and behavior toward the coworker.
1
Drawing upon social comparison theory (Festinger, 1954), we
propose that a coworker's higher LMX, reflected by the individual's
selfperceived upward LMXSC and the coworker's otherperceived
downward LMXSC, will constitute an unfavorable comparison
outcome for the individual because the former LMXSC captures the
lower status perceived by the individual himself or herself, and the
latter LMXSC assigns to the individual a lower status in a hierarchical
LMX relationship. In this respect, social comparison theory (Festinger,
1954) suggests that in response to disadvantaged and unfavorable
comparison outcomes, the individual may develop negative hostile
emotions (Buunk, Collins, Taylor, Van Yperen, & Dakof, 1990; Heider,
1958) and may retaliate toward the coworker (e.g., a harmful act;
Lam, Van der Vegt, Walter, & Huang, 2011), depending on the extent
to which the individual contrasts or assimilates himself or herself with
that coworker (i.e., by focusing either on relational differences or on
commonalities; Mussweiler, Rüter, & Epstude, 2004).
More importantly, such contrastive or assimilative processes are
greatly shaped by contextual factors (Greenberg, AshtonJames, &
Ashkanasy, 2007; Lam et al., 2011). Along this line, literature on both
LMX and justice climate has suggested that a particularly salient
contextual factor in team settings that may determine employees'
responses to an unfavorable comparison outcome is procedural justice
(PJ) climate (i.e., a teamlevel cognition regarding how fairly the team
as a whole is treated procedurally
2
; Erdogan & Bauer, 2010; Naumann
& Bennett, 2000). Because supervisors are typical representatives of
an organization and also are often involved in communicating the value
and implementing the practice of the PJ climate to all team members,
employees tend to refer to PJ climate to make sense of an authority
figure's practice, such as a supervisor's exercise of LMX differentiation
(Erdogan & Bauer, 2010; Hollensbe, Khazanchi, & Masterson, 2008).
We therefore expect that the PJ climate operates as a sensemaking
mechanism, shaping the extent to which the coworker's higher LMX
is legitimate, and also determining whether the individual will contrast
or assimilate with the coworker.
Combining the identificationcontrast model of social comparison
theory (Buunk & Ybema, 1997; Mussweiler et al., 2004) and the
symbolic model of justice climate (Lin & Leung, 2014), we propose that
the relationship between a coworker's higher LMX and an individual's
hostile responses toward that coworker hinges on the PJ climate.
Specifically, unfair team procedures (i.e., in a low PJ climate) would
provide the individual with the information that the coworker's higher
LMX standing, greater resources, and influence obtained simply on the
basis of a better relationship with the supervisor do not procedurally
match, in a just way, his or her efforts spent on work. Given that the
social and economic costs of undermining supervisors are too high
(Mitchell & Ambrose, 2007), we argue that this coworker, who is a
direct competitor to the individual in a triadic relationship, would be
held responsible for creating the unfavorable LMX comparison
outcome. Such external blaming will guide the individual to contrast
himself or herself with the coworker and to develop a negative opinion
and hostile reactions toward him or her. In contrast, when team
procedures are fair (i.e., there is a high PJ climate), the individual tends
to view that the coworker's higher LMX as being legitimate and
justified, and thus the individual is more likely to assimilate and less
likely to feel hostile toward the higher LMX coworker. That compari-
son outcome is proper when implemented on the basis of the
principles of justice. Furthermore, a handful of research suggests that
hostility (e.g., anger, disgust, and hostility) is a justicerelated emotion
for outcome unfavorability (Barclay, Skarlicki, & Pugh, 2005; Brockner,
2002) and can provoke interpersonal harmful acts (i.e., behaviors that
go against the legitimate interests of another individual in the organiza-
tion; cf. Venkataramani & Dalal, 2007, p. 952; Glomb, 2002). To
1
In the context of a dyadic individualcoworker relationship, upward social com-
parison is made in relation to someone of a higher LMX standing and downward
social comparison is made in relation to someone of a lower LMX standing in a
team (Buunk et al., 1990; Festinger, 1954). We therefore use selfperceived
upward LMXSCto refer to an individual employee's perception that a
coworker's LMX is higher than the LMX that he or she has with the supervisor,
and we use otherperceived downward LMXSCto refer to a coworker's per-
ception that his or her own LMX is higher than that of the individual with the
supervisor.
2
In the present research, we focus only on the use of PJ climate as an important
boundary condition, for four reasons. First, PJ has been proposed and specified
as a primary predictor for understanding the psychological basis of whether peo-
ple socially identify with a workgroup in the group engagement model (Tyler &
Blader, 2000). Second, PJ has received more substantial research attention than
distributive justice and interactional justice have, because of its stronger predic-
tive ability for a wide range of employee attitudes and behaviors (cf. metaana-
lytical review; CohenCharash & Spector, 2001; Colquitt, 2001). Third,
employee reactions to distributive justice can be influenced by procedural justice
because the fairness of the actual resource distribution is largely determined by
fair decisionmaking procedures and processes (Du, Choi, & Hashem, 2012).
Finally, there is a high degree of conceptual overlap among interactional justice,
distributive justice, and LMX because leaders rely on the quality of relationships
with their subordinates when they distribute organizational resources and psy-
chological support (Erdogan & Bauer, 2010).
TSE ET AL.923

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT