Dynamic effects of personal initiative on engagement and exhaustion: The role of mood, autonomy, and support

AuthorNerina L. Jimmieson,Antje Schmitt,Cort W. Rudolph,Hannes Zacher
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1002/job.2277
Published date01 January 2019
Date01 January 2019
SPECIAL ISSUE ARTICLE
Dynamic effects of personal initiative on engagement and
exhaustion: The role of mood, autonomy, and support
Hannes Zacher
1
|Antje Schmitt
2
|Nerina L. Jimmieson
3
|Cort W. Rudolph
4
1
Institute of Psychology, Leipzig University,
Leipzig, Germany
2
Department of Psychology, University of
Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
3
School of Management, Queensland
University of Technology, Brisbane, QLD,
Australia
4
Department of Psychology, Saint Louis
University, St. Louis, Missouri, U.S.A.
Correspondence
Hannes Zacher, Institute of Psychology,
Leipzig University, Neumarkt 919, 04109
Leipzig, Germany.
Email: hannes.zacher@unileipzig.de
Summary
Researchers have neglected dynamic effects of proactive behavior on occupational wellbeing.
We investigated effects of change in personal initiative over 6 months on changes in emotional
engagement and exhaustion over the following 6 months. On the basis of the controlprocess
theory on affect, we hypothesized that changes in positive and negative moods mediate these
effects conditional upon employees' level of perceived organizational support. On the basis of
action regulation theory, we assumed that change in job autonomy also acts as a mediator. Data
came from 297 employees who responded to 3 surveys separated by 6month intervals. Results
of latent change score modeling showed that change in personal initiative negatively predicted
change in positive mood and, when perceived organizational support was low, positively pre-
dicted change in negative mood. In addition, change in personal initiative positively predicted
change in job autonomy. Change in personal initiative had negative indirect effects on change
in emotional engagement, and positive indirect effects on change in emotional exhaustion
through changes in positive and negative moods (but not through change in job autonomy). A
reverse causal model did not yield significant indirect effects. Overall, these findings suggest that
an increase in proactive behavior can have detrimental effects on occupational wellbeing.
KEYWORDS
engagement, exhaustion, latent change score, personalinitiative, proactivity
1|INTRODUCTION
Proactivebehavior involvesemployees' selfinitiatedand futureoriented
effortsto change their work environmentor themselves in positive ways
(Bindl & Parker, 2011; Crant, 2000; Grant & Ashford, 2008; Parker &
Collins, 2010). In the context of increasingly complex and uncertain
workplaces and careers, research on proactive behavior and its conse-
quences for employees and organizations has substantially grown over
the past two decades (Griffin, Neal, & Parker, 2007; Parker & Bindl,
2017). For instance, two metaanalytic reviews showed that proactive
behavior is positively related tojob performance, even after controlling
for the Big Five personality traits (Thomas, Whitman, & Viswesvaran,
2010; Tornau& Frese, 2013).
Although associations between proactive behavior and perfor-
mancerelated outcomes are now well established, organizational
scholars have so far largely neglected potential effects of proactive
behavior on occupational wellbeing outcomes (for two recent excep-
tions, see Fay & Hüttges, 2017; Strauss, Parker, & O'Shea, 2017).
Furthermore, the potential mechanisms and boundary conditions of
effects of proactivebehavior on occupationalwellbeing outcomeshave
not received muchresearch attention (Cangiano & Parker, 2016). Most
previous studies have used crosssectional betweenperson designs,
and, therefore,our knowledge of how withinperson changein proactive
behaviorimpacts withinpersonchange in occupationalwellbeing is lim-
ited (see Eschleman & LaHuis, 2014).Bolino, Valcea, and Harvey (2010)
argued that longitudinal studies on proactive behavior are needed,
because proactive behavior might have negative consequences for
employees' wellbeing over time (e.g., several months). The few existing
longitudinal studies (i.e., with three or more measurement waves;
Ployhart & Vandenberg, 2010) on proactive behavior did not examine
indicatorsof occupationalwellbeing as predictorsor outcomes of proac-
tive behavior(Fay & Sonnentag, 2002;Frese, Garst, & Fay, 2007;Griffin,
Parker, & Mason,2010; Li, Fay, Frese, Harms, & Gao, 2014). Moreover,
previous studieshave focused on the extent to which betweenperson
differences in proactive behavior are associated with betweenperson
differencesin outcomes, and not on how withinperson changein proac-
tive behavior predicts subsequent withinperson changes in outcomes.
Whereas the former approach can lead to a better understanding of
Received: 22 November 2016 Revised: 12 February 2018 Accepted: 20 February 2018
DOI: 10.1002/job.2277
38 © 2018 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J Organ Behav. 2019;40:3858.wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/job
how and why proactive employees differ from their less proactive
colleagues, the latter approach allows firmer conclusions regarding
causality, because it investigates how change in proactive behavior
impacts changesin other variables, such as occupationalwellbeing (see
Hamaker, 2012;Molenaar, 2004; M. Wang et al., 2017).
In this study, we address the limitations of previous research by
integrating affectbased, motivational, and resourcebased perspec-
tives on the consequences of proactive behavior (Cangiano, Bindl, &
Parker, 2017; Cangiano & Parker, 2016; Frese et al., 2007). Specifically,
we draw on propositions of the controlprocess theory on affect (Car-
ver & Scheier, 1990) and action regulation theory (Frese & Zapf, 1994)
to explain potential indirect effects of change in personal initiative
over 6 months on changes in emotional engagement and emotional
exhaustion over the following 6 months. Personal initiative is a broad
form of proactive work behavior that is selfstarting, future oriented,
and overcomes barriers to achieve goals (Frese & Fay, 2001). For
example, employees show high personal initiative when they seek
opportunities to improve their work environment and are persistent
in the face of barriers and setbacks that may occur while implementing
changes (Frese, Fay, Hilburger, Leng, & Tag, 1997).
High emotional engagement and low emotional exhaustion are
two important indicators of occupational wellbeing (Bakker, Albrecht,
& Leiter, 2011) that have been shown to be associated with high
performance, reduced turnover, and general wellbeing (Bakker &
Bal, 2010; Hakanen & Schaufeli, 2012; Wright & Cropanzano, 1998).
Emotional engagement involves the investment of affective energies
into the work role (Kahn, 1990; Rich, LePine, & Crawford, 2010).
Emotionally engaged employees are enthusiastic and energetic intheir
job (Bakker, Schaufeli, Leiter, & Taris, 2008). Emotional exhaustion is a
key dimension of the job burnout construct and entails feelings of
being emotionally depleted, fatigued, and overextended by one's work
(Maslach & Jackson, 1981; Maslach, Schaufeli, & Leiter, 2001).
Emotional engagement and emotional exhaustion are moderately to
strongly negatively related, but distinct concepts (Bakker et al., 2011;
Cole, Walter, Bedeian, & O'Boyle, 2012). By including these two out-
comes, we adopt a balanced approach that focuses on both positive
and negative aspects of occupational wellbeing (Hakanen, Bakker, &
Schaufeli, 2006).
To advance research on the wellbeing consequences of proactive
behavior, Cangiano and Parker (2016) proposed that proactive behav-
ior can influence occupational wellbeing outcomes via a resource
generation pathway and a resourcedepletion pathway, and that the
strength of these effects depends on contextual (e.g., support) and
individual (e.g., intrinsic motivation) factors (see also Cangiano, 2017).
Research based on the job demandsresources model (Demerouti,
Bakker, Nachreiner, & Schaufeli, 2001) similarly suggests a motiva-
tional process that results in work engagement and wellbeing, and
an energetical process that leads to burnout and ill health (Hakanen
et al., 2006). Furthermore, Cangiano et al. (2017) suggested that
proactive behavior is not only influenced by affective experiences
(e.g., Bindl, Parker, Totterdell, & HaggerJohnson, 2012) but that pro-
active behavior also can have important consequences for affective
experiences. Consistent with Cangiano and Parker's research, we
hypothesize that change in personal initiative has indirect effects on
subsequent changes in emotional engagement and exhaustion. Specif-
ically, we expect that these effects are mediated by changes in positive
and negative moods and job autonomy, and that the effects of change
in personal initiative on changes in positive and negative moods are
moderated by perceived organizational support (POS).
Our conceptual model is shown in Figure 1. We do not hypothe-
size overall positive or negative effects of change in personal initiative
on changes in emotional engagement and exhaustion. The reason for
this is that the different positive and negative indirect pathways are
likely to cancel each other out and lead to weak overall effects of
change in personal initiative on changes in occupational wellbeing
(Cangiano & Parker, 2016). However, we outline boundary conditions
under which the positive indirect pathway might outweigh the nega-
tive indirect pathway or vice versa, such that a change in personal ini-
tiative has positive or negative overall effects on changes in
occupational wellbeing outcomes.
Positive and negative moods, two of the mediators in our model,
entail relatively enduring experiences of pleasant (e.g., excited)
and unpleasant (e.g ., distressed) affective stat es, respectively (Warr,
Barter, & Brownbridge, 1983; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988).
Although emotions ar e short lived and intense react ions to specific
events, moods typica lly have a longer duration (e .g., several days,
weeks, or months), are l ess intense, and are more generali zed in
focus (Barsade & Gibson, 2007; Brief & Weiss, 2002). Moods fur-
ther have to be distinguished from even more stable interindividual
differences in trait affectivity, or the dispositional tendency to
experience positive o r negative emotions (Barsade & Gibson,
2007; Cangiano et al., 2017).
FIGURE 1 Conceptual model. Δ= change in; T = time. Dashed lines indicate conditional (i.e., moderator) effects
ZACHER ET AL.39

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT