Control Attitudes toward Drug Use as a Function of Paternalistic and Moralistic Principles

Published date01 January 2002
AuthorStelios Stylianou
Date01 January 2002
DOI10.1177/002204260203200106
Subject MatterArticle
© 2002 BY THE JOURNAL OF DRUG ISSUES
JOURNAL OF DRUG ISSUES 0022-0426/02/01 119-152
CONTROL ATTITUDES TOWARD DRUG USE AS A
FUNCTION OF PATERNALISTIC AND MORALISTIC
PRINCIPLES
STELIOS STYLIANOU
Perceptions of crime seriousness have been studied since the 1960s.
Characteristics of acts affecting these perceptions have been identified, and the
degree of agreement in seriousness judgments has been examined for a variety of
behaviors. The present study extends this inquiry by investigating how perceptions
of self-harm and perceptions of immorality shape attitudes toward the control of
drug use. These attitudes and perceptions were measured for six widely known
drugs – alcohol, cigarettes, cocaine, heroin, LSD, and marijuana – using an
electronic mail survey of university students. Univariate analysis shows dissensus
rather than consensus in attitudes and perceptions and that, with the exception of
marijuana, control attitudes toward drug use reflect the existing legal code.
Multivariate analysis shows that perceptions of self-harm and perceptions of
immorality are moderately to highly correlated and that control attitudes are strongly
affected by perceptions of self-harm and perceptions of immorality.
INTRODUCTION
The study of popular perceptions of crime seriousness was introduced in
sociology by Sellin and Wolfgang (1964). Since then, numerous studies have been
published in an effort to illuminate how people perceive crime. Of central concern
has been the study of characteristics of criminal acts and how they affect seriousness
perceptions. Additionally, in the context of the consensus-conflict debate, there
has been interest in measuring how much agreement there is among individuals
__________
Stelios Stylianou, Ph.D., is Assistant Professor of Sociology at Intercollege, Lefkosia, Cyprus. He
received his Ph.D. from the Department of Sociology at the University of Washington, Seattle. His
research focuses on the moral structure of social control. In particular, he has studied control attitudes
toward victimless behaviors, predominantly drug use and sexual deviance. Direct correspondence to
Stelios Stylianou, Department of Liberal Arts, Intercollege, 46 Makedonitissas Avenue, PO BOX 24005,
1700 Lefkosia, Cyprus. E-mail: stelios@u.washington.edu
STYLIANOU
120 JOURNAL OF DRUG ISSUES
and groups regarding the perceived seriousness of crime. However, few attempts
have been made to systematically model perceived crime seriousness as a function
of moral principles. The specification of such principles has been a concern in
normative philosophy, where libertarian, liberal/paternalistic, and moralistic
doctrines have been advanced and debated. The extent to which these philosophical
doctrines are reflected in the popular discourse on social control deserves attention.
In this study, I address this issue in the substantive area of drug use by testing
hypotheses that predict paternalistic and moralistic reasoning in public opinion. In
particular, I model an alternative measure of seriousness perceptions (control
attitudes) as a function of two components: perceptions of self-harm and perceptions
of immorality. Additionally, I hypothesize that the empirical distribution of control
attitudes will resemble the current legal code and that perceptions of self-harm and
perceptions of immorality will be correlated. I test these hypotheses using data
from an electronic mail survey of a random sample of students at a large university
in the United States.
I begin by reviewing the relevant empirical literature and then outlining the
philosophical principles modeled in the present research.
REVIEW OF THE EMPIRICAL LITERATURE
The empirical literature on public perceptions of crime seriousness has addressed
a wide range of conceptual elements and has focused on a large number of behaviors.
One thing we have learned from this literature is that the consequences of an act are
the most important determinants of seriousness perceptions. Acts causing bodily
harm are generally perceived as the most serious, followed by acts causing property
loss or property damage (Sellin & Wolfgang, 1964; Rossi, Waite, Bose, & Berk,
1974; Rossi & Henry, 1980; Schrager & Short, 1980; McCleary, O’Neil, Epperlein,
Jones, & Gray, 1981; Cullen, Link, & Polanzi, 1982; Evans & Scott, 1984; Wolfgang,
Figlio, Tracy, & Singer, 1985; Warr, 1989).
Consistently, victimless crimes are generally rated the least serious (Wolfgang
et al., 1985; Blum-West, 1985; Cullen et al., 1982). However, “victimlessness”
does not seem to be a unidimensional construct (Abrams & Della-Fave, 1976).
Research that included victimless crimes with a strong moral element, for example,
has shown that perceptions of immorality significantly affect perceived seriousness
(Newman, 1976; Evans & Scott, 1984). Regarding drug use, there has been no
systematic study of the perceived seriousness of the use of different substances.
Using heroin or LSD appears in some surveys as very a serious offense. Rossi et
al. (1974), for example, asked a sample of Baltimore residents to assess the
seriousness of 140 offenses. In their results, using heroin ranked 28th in seriousness
and LSD use 65th. Most other victimless offenses (such as prostitution and
CONTROL ATTITUDES TOWARD DRUG USE
121WINTER 2002
homosexuality) ranked much lower. Cullen et al. (1982) used the same list of
offenses in a survey of residents of Macomb, Illinois. Using heroin and using LSD
ranked 58th and 83rd (out of 140) respectively, while most other victimless crimes
ranked below 120. The seriousness attributed to the use of heroin and LSD becomes
more pronounced, if we consider that in both these studies some violent behaviors
ranked about the same, or even less serious. Forcible rape of a former spouse, for
example, ranked 58th (same as heroin use) in Rossi et al. (1974) and 62nd (much less
serious than heroin use and slightly more serious than LSD use) in Cullen et al.
(1982).
The issue of consensus has also been widely researched and discussed. Each
behavior can be placed on the consensus-conflict continuum (Hagan, 1994; Hagan,
Silva, & Simpson, 1977) based on how much agreement there is about how serious
it is. Various types of consensus were defined by Rossi and Berk (1985). Regarding
the mathematical properties of the distribution of scores, “absolute consensus” refers
to agreement on the parameters of the distribution of seriousness perceptions (same
absolute scores) and “relative consensus” refers to agreement on the mathematical
form (function or shape) of the distribution.1 Both intraculturally and cross-
culturally, relative consensus has been one of the most persistent findings in crime
seriousness research (Rossi & Henry, 1980; Cullen, Link, Travis, & Wozniak, 1985).
With respect to generality, “global consensus” refers to agreement on all normative
domains (all norms included), while “local consensus” refers to agreement on a
single norm (for example, marijuana use). Findings suggest that local consensus is
very often present. Miethe (1984) presented four types of consensus based on the
cross-tabulation of the absolute-relative dimension and the global2-local dimension.
Using data from Rossi et al. (1974), he found evidence of global relative but not of
global absolute consensus. He also found local absolute consensus for violent,
white-collar, and public order offenses. Carlson and Williams (1993) also showed
that consensus exists with respect to crimes involving injury or loss of life.
Victimless crimes produce more dissensus rather than consensus (Miethe, 1984;
Carlson & Williams, 1993).
Cross-culturally, early replications of Sellin and Wolfgang’s (1964) study were
done in Canada by Akman and Normandeau (Normandeau, 1966; Akman &
Normandeau, 1967, 1968). The conclusions of these studies are highly consistent
with Sellin and Wolfgang (1964). A number of studies followed in the early 1970s,
reporting both agreement and disagreement between Western and non-Western
cultures (Velez-Diaz & Megagree, 1971; Hsu, 1973; Carss & Whitrod, 1974). In a
large-scale comparative survey of six countries, Newman (1976) showed that relative
consensus exists cross-culturally with respect to behaviors that are generally ranked
high on the seriousness scales (offenses involving bodily injury, followed by those

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT