Causal Inference in Accounting Research

Date01 May 2016
AuthorIAN D. GOW,DAVID F. LARCKER,PETER C. REISS
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/1475-679X.12116
Published date01 May 2016
DOI: 10.1111/1475-679X.12116
Journal of Accounting Research
Vol. 54 No. 2 May 2016
Printed in U.S.A.
Causal Inference in Accounting
Research
IAN D. GOW,
DAVID F. LARCKER,
AND PETER C. REISS
ABSTRACT
This paper examines the approaches accounting researchers adopt to draw
causal inferences using observational (or nonexperimental) data. The vast
majority of accounting research papers draw causal inferences notwithstand-
ing the well-known difficulties in doing so. While some recent papers seek to
use quasi-experimental methods to improve causal inferences, these methods
also make strong assumptions that are not always fully appreciated. We be-
lieve that accounting research would benefit from more in-depth descriptive
research, including a greater focus on the study of causal mechanisms (or
causal pathways) and increased emphasis on the structural modeling of the
phenomena of interest. We argue these changes offer a practical path forward
for rigorous accounting research.
JEL codes: C18; C190; C51; M40; M41
Keywords: Causal inference; accounting research; quasi-experimental
methods; structural modeling
1. Introduction
There is perhaps no more controversial practice in social and biomedi-
cal research than drawing inferences from observational data. Despite ...
Harvard Business School; Rock Center for Corporate Governance, Stanford Graduate
School of Business.
Accepted by Philip Berger.We are grateful to our discussants, Christian Hansen and Miguel
Minutti-Meza, and participants at the 2015 JAR Conference for helpful feedback. We also
thank seminar participants at London Business School, Karthik Balakrishnan, Robert Ka-
plan, Christian Leuz, Alexander Ljungqvist, Eugene Soltes, Daniel Taylor, Robert Verrecchia,
Charles Wang, and Anastasia Zakolyukina for comments.
477
Copyright C, University of Chicago on behalf of the Accounting Research Center,2016
478 I.D.GOW,D.F.LARCKER,AND P.C.REISS
problems, observational data are widely available in many scientific fields
and are routinely used to draw inferences about the causal impact of inter-
ventions. The key issue, therefore, is not whether such studies should be
done, but how they may be done well. (Berk [1999, p.95])
Most empirical research in accounting relies on observational (or non-
experimental) data. This paper evaluates the different approaches account-
ing researchers adopt to draw causal inferences from observational data.1
Our discussion draws on developments in fields such as statistics, econo-
metrics, and epidemiology. The goal of this paper is to identify areas for
improvement and suggest how empirical accounting research can improve
inferences drawn from observational data.
The importance of causal inference in accounting research is clear from
the research questions that accounting researchers seek to answer. Most
long-standing questions in accounting research are causal: Does conser-
vatism affect the terms of loan contracts? Do higher quality earnings reports
lead to lower information asymmetry? Did International Financial Report-
ing Standards cause an increase in liquidity in the jurisdictions that adopted
them? Do managerial incentives lead to managerial misstatements in finan-
cial reports? The accounting researchers focus on causal inference, which
is consistent with the view that “the most interesting research in social sci-
ence is about questions of cause and effect” (Angrist and Pischke [2008,
p. 3]). Simply documenting descriptive correlations provides little basis for
understanding what would happen should circumstances change, whereas
using data to make inferences that support or refute broader theories could
facilitate these kinds of predictions.
To provide insights into what is actually done in empirical accounting
research, we examined all papers published in three leading accounting
journals in 2014. While accounting researchers are aware of problems that
can arise from the use of observational data to draw causal inferences, we
found that most papers still seek to draw such inferences. Making causal
inferences requires strong assumptions about the causal relations among
variables. For example, estimating the causal effect of Xon Yrequires
that the researcher has controlled for variables that could confound esti-
mates of such effects. Section 2 provides an overview of causal inference
using causal diagrams as a framework for thinking about the subtle issues
involved. We believe that these diagrams are also very useful for communi-
cating the cause-and-effect logic underlying regression analyses that use ob-
servational data. Nonetheless, difficulties identifying, measuring, and con-
trolling for all possible confounding variables have led many to question
causal inferences drawn from observational data.
Recently, some social scientists have held out hope that better re-
search designs and statistical methods can increase the credibility of causal
1Thus, our focus is on what Bloomfield, Nelson, and Soltes [2016] call “archival studies.”
Floyd and List [2016] discuss opportunities for researchers to use experiments in accounting
research.
CAUSAL INFERENCE IN ACCOUNTING RESEARCH 479
inferences. For example, Angrist and Pischke [2010] suggest that “empir-
ical microeconomics has experienced a credibility revolution, with a con-
sequent increase in policy relevance and scientific impact.” Angrist and
Pischke [2010, p. 26] argue that such “improvement has come mostly
from better research designs, either by virtue of outright experimenta-
tion or through the well-founded and careful implementation of quasi-
experimental methods.” Our survey of research published in 2014 finds 5
studies claiming to study natural experiments (or “exogenous shocks”) and
10 studies using instrumental variables (IVs). Although these numbers sug-
gest that quasi-experimental methods are infrequently used in accounting
research, we believe their use will increase in the future.2
Section 3 evaluates the use of quasi-experimental methods in account-
ing research. Quasi-experimental methods produce credible estimates of
causal effects only under very strong maintained assumptions about the
model and data. For example, variations in treatments are rarely random,
the list of controls rarely exhaustive, and instruments do not always sat-
isfy the necessary inclusion and exclusion restrictions. We explain some of
these concerns using causal diagrams. In general, it appears that the as-
sumptions required to apply quasi-experimental methods are unlikely to
be satisfied by observational data in most empirical accounting research
settings.
Ultimately, we believe that accounting research needs to recognize the
stringent assumptions that need to be maintained to apply statistical meth-
ods to derive estimates of causal effects for observational data. Statistical
methods alone cannot solve the inference issues that arise in observational
data. The second part of the paper (sections 4 and 5) identifies approaches
that can provide a plausible framework for guiding future accounting
research. Specifically:
rThere should be an increased emphasis on the study of causal mecha-
nisms, that is, the “pathways” through which claimed causal effects are
propagated. We believe that evidence on the actions and beliefs of indi-
viduals and institutions can bolster causal claims based on associations,
even absent compelling estimates of the causal effects. We also suggest
that more careful modeling of phenomena, using structural modeling
or causal diagrams, can help to identify plausible mechanisms that war-
rant further study.
rCausal diagrams are a useful tool for conveying the key elements of
a structural model and can also act as a middle-level stand-in when
structural modeling of a phenomenon is infeasible.3
2We use the term “quasi-experimental” methods to refer to those methods that have a plau-
sible claim to “as if” random assignment to treatment conditions. The term “as if” is used by
Dunning [2012] to acknowledge the fact that assignment is not random in such settings, but
is claimed to be as if random assignment had occurred.
3“Middle-level” here refers to the placement of causal diagrams between relatively informal
verbal reasoning and the rigors of a structural model.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT