American Public Opinion About Prisons

Published date01 September 2014
AuthorKevin H. Wozniak
Date01 September 2014
DOI10.1177/0734016814529968
Subject MatterArticles
Article
American Public Opinion
About Prisons
Kevin H. Wozniak
1
Abstract
Politicians in the United States frequently cite public anger when they attack ‘‘country-club’
conditions in prisons. Despite the ubiquity of this rhetoric, it is backed by limited empirical support.
Few studies measure American citizens’ attitudes toward prison conditions, and even fewer studies
do so with data from random samples of the population. To address this limitation in our knowledge,
I measure public perceptions of life in prison and opinions about the appropriate severity of pun-
ishment in prison with data from an original public opinion survey administered to a national sample
of the U.S. population. I find that a plurality of respondents both perceive life in prison to be
unpleasant and voice the opinion that life in prison should be harsher still. I contextualize these
findings within the broader study of public opinion about punishment.
Keywords
prison, corrections, crime policy, public opinion, survey methods, politics
Introduction
The Politics of Punishment
One of the most significant changes to American society over the past 40 years has been the rise of
the ‘‘penal harm movement’’ and ‘‘mass incarceration’’ (Clear, 1994; Garland, 2001; Gottschalk,
2006). Beginning in the 1970s, policy makers abandoned the rehabilitation-centric paradigm that
dominated corrections in the 1960s in favor of policies that lengthened prison sentences, abolished
parole, and curtailed judicial discretion. These policy changes sparked an exponential increase in the
nation’s incarceration rate, culminating in the present reality that the United States imprisons over
two million of its citizens and has an incarceration rate several times higher than that of any other
industrialized nation in the world (Blumstein & Beck, 1999; Warren, Gelb, Horowitz, & Riordan,
2008).
Beyond expanding the scope of punishment, advocates of ‘‘tough on crime’’ policies also worked
to increase the harshness of punishment. Politicians framed criminal justice as a ‘‘zero-sum game’
in which any policy choice that was good for offenders was bad for victims of crime. Under this
1
Department of Sociology, University of Massachusetts Boston, Boston, MA, USA
Corresponding Author:
KevinH. Wozniak, Department of Sociology,University of MassachusettsBoston, 100 Morrissey Blvd.,Boston, MA 02125, USA.
Email: kevin.wozniak@umb.edu
Criminal Justice Review
2014, Vol. 39(3) 305-324
ª2014 Georgia State University
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0734016814529968
cjr.sagepub.com
logic, the only way to deliver justice to crime victims was to make offenders suffer (Simon, 2007;
Zimring, Hawkins, & Kamin, 2001). A notable manifestation of this principle was the ‘‘no-frills’’
movement of the early 1990s (Finn, 1996). One survey found that about 60%of state departments
of corrections raised the requirements for inmates to participate in programs or eliminated privileges
in prison, such as free weights, radios, and TVs. Even Congress enacted the ‘‘No-Frills Prison Act’
in 1996, which was designed to eliminate ‘‘luxurious’’ conditions in federal prisons (Wunder, 1995).
Importantly, many wardens believed that ‘‘the single most powerful driving force behind [no-
frills bills was] legislators who believe the legislation helps them to get elected or reelected by offer-
ing the impression that they are tough on crime’’ (Finn, 1996, p. 36; see also Johnson, Bennett, &
Flanagan, 1997). Politicians and the media believed that the public angrily resented conditions in
prisons that they perceived to be like a resort or country club (Bidinotto, 1994; Welch, Weber, &
Edwards, 2000). As an illustrative example, Boulard (1995, p. 24) quotes a Colorado state senator
who said, ‘‘There is a concern that crime is out of control and that the people responsible for it come
to prison and live the good life. It is now up to us as lawmakers to address those concerns and see
what needs to be improved upon or taken away or just changed’’ (see also Crist, 1996; Gottschalk,
2006; Lynch, 2010, for other examples of politicians attacking prison amenities). Roberts and Hough
(2005) argue that the public perception of life in prison as ‘‘easy’’ fueled penal populism and pol-
iticians’ support for longer, harsher sentences. Beyond the ‘‘no-frills’’ movement, politicians fre-
quently cited punitive public opinion as their justification for supporting all the penal policies
that gave rise to mass incarceration (Beckett, 1997), and a growing body of research indicates that
a relationship does exist between mass public opinion and criminal justice system outcomes (Enns,
2014; Nicholson-Crotty, Peterson, & Ramirez, 2009).
In spite of the politicized debate over prison living conditions, the ongoing court battles about
overcrowding, and the simple fact that the United States imprisons more people than any other
industrialized country in the world, we possess surprisingly little data that actually measure citizens’
opinions about prisons. Furthermore, the few studies that measure these attitudes possess limited
generalizability. From a scientific point of view, we do not properly understand the extent to which
politicians’ claims of public antipathy toward country-club prisons are supported by empirical evi-
dence (if at all).
Public Opinion About Prisons: A Dearth of Research
Most of what criminologists know about public opinion is pertinent but tangential to the present
study. Several studies found that pluralities of citizens in countries around the world classify reha-
bilitation as a more important purpose for imprisonment than retribution (e.g., Cullen, Fisher, &
Applegate, 2000; Cullen, Pealer, Fisher, Applegate, & Santana, 2002; Flanagan & Caulfield,
1984; Roberts & Hough, 2005). Other surveys find that a majority of U.S. citizens support educa-
tional and vocational programs in prisons, as well as the ability to earn good time credit toward a
sentence reduction (Flanagan, 1996; Haghighi & Lopez, 1998). While these findings reveal what
people want prisons to do, these studies do not measure public perceptions of and attitudes about
the prison environment that formed the political core of the no-frills movement. As such, they shed
no light on the empirical question of whether or not people perceive and resent so-called country-
club conditions in prisons.
Our most in-depth information about American public perceptions of life in prison comes from
qualitative focus groups and/or interviews. Most of these studies do find that participants perceive
prison life to be too easy and comfortable for inmates, and many people resent inmates’ access to
amenities, like TV and exercise equipment (Cook & Powell, 2003; Doble, 1987; Doble, Immerwahr,
& Richardson, 1991; Doble & Klein, 1989; Sasson, 1995). This punitive view is the majority opinion
among participants in most, but not all, focus groups. For example, researchers in Massachusetts
306 Criminal Justice Review 39(3)

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT