Who gets the benefit of the doubt? The impact of causal reasoning depth on how violations of gender stereotypes are evaluated

Date01 March 2018
Published date01 March 2018
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1002/job.2218
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Who gets the benefit of the doubt? The impact of causal
reasoning depth on how violations of gender stereotypes are
evaluated
Steffen Keck
1
|Linda Babcock
2
1
Department of Business Administration,
University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
2
Department of Social and Decision Sciences,
Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh,
Pennsylvania, U.S.A.
Correspondence
Steffen Keck, Department of Business
Administration, University of Vienna, Oskar
Morgenstern Platz, Vienna 1090, Austria.
Email: steffen.keck@univie.ac.at
Summary
A large body of research demonstrates that women encounter severe penalties for violating gen-
der stereotypes. In this paper, we explore the conditions under which the reverse is truewhen
being subject to a stereotype can actually benefit a woman compared to a man who is not subject
to the same stereotype. In particular, we suggest that in situations of causal ambiguity
uncertainty about the reasons that a behavior occurreddifferences in how men and women
are evaluated will be moderated by the extent to which observers engage in a low or a high level
of deliberative causal reasoning. In 3 experimental studies, participants were asked to make judg-
ments about an employee who violated a female gender stereotype by failing to provide help to a
coworker when asked to do so, but the reasons for this behavior were unclear. When participants
were prompted to engage in deliberative causal reasoning, women were evaluated more positively
than men, but not in the absence of such a prompt. Moreover, when participants did engage in
deliberative causal reasoning, the more positive evaluations of women compared to men were
driven by participants0beliefs that women0s behavior was due more to situational constraints than
the same behavior by men.
KEYWORDS
causal attributions, gender stereotypes,social accounts
1|INTRODUCTION
Gender stereotypes are pervasive, easily activated, and affect
individuals0judgments even outside of their conscious control
(Dunning & Sherman, 1997; Rudman & Glick, 2001). Prior research
has shown that women who violate female gender stereotypes in the
workplace encounter backlash in the form of more severe social and
economic penalties than men who exhibit the same behavior. For
example, female employees who behave in a dominant manner
(Bowles, Babcock, & Lai, 2007; Eagly, Makhijani, & Klonsky, 1992;
Lewis, 2000) or fail to carry out communal actions such as helping a
fellow coworker (Heilman & Chen, 2005) are judged more harshly than
male employees. In this paper, we examine conditions under which the
reverse is truethat is, when being subject to a stereotype can actually
benefit a woman who engages in nonstereotypical behavior, relative to
a man who is not subject to the stereotype.
In particular, we demonstrate that in situations of causal ambiguity
when there is uncertainty about the reasons that a behavior occurred
evaluations of noncommunal behavior by men and women will
strongly depend on the extent to which observers engage in either a
low or a high level of deliberative causal reasoning. We conducted
three experiments that manipulated depth of causal reasoning either
directly or indirectly by enhancing the salience of the causal ambiguity.
Our findings show that when observers engage in more deliberative
causal reasoning, women who behave in a noncommunal manner are
judged more positively than men who behave in the same manner. This
is because, when observers think more deeply about the reasons for
the noncommunal behavior, they make more situational attributions
for women than for men. In other words, women are more likely than
men to get the benefit of the doubt.In contrast, when observers
engage in a low level of deliberative causal reasoning, their attributions
are predominately dispositional, regardless of the actor0s gender. In
this case, our findings are more in line with those of previous research,
where women were judged more harshly than men.
Our findings have several important implications. As shown by a
large body of prior research, causal ambiguity and the need to make
causal attributions are pervasive in the workplace and have the poten-
tial to strongly shape how different types of workplace behavior are
Received: 5 March 2015 Revised: 26 June 2017 Accepted: 7 July 2017
DOI: 10.1002/job.2218
276 Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J Organ Behav. 2018;39:276291.wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/job
perceived (e.g., Burton, Taylor, & Barber, 2014; Kim, Dirks, Cooper, &
Ferrin, 2006; LePine & Van Dyne, 2001; Martinko & Gardner, 1987).
However, despite the considerable attention paid to causal attribu-
tions in the workplace on the one hand and backlash against violations
of gender stereotypes on the other, the role of causal reasoning depth
has not been integrated into studying these phenomena. Our study fills
this gap by exploring how causal reasoning depth impacts the evalua-
tion of counterstereotypical behavior in the presence of causal ambi-
guity. In doing so, our research makes an important contribution to the
literature on gender stereotypes by building a more comprehensive
account of the impact of stereotypes on observer evaluations and
outlining the moderating role of causal reasoning processes, which
can even lead to a reversal of previous findings. Moreover, our findings
add to previous work on the effect of attributions on perceptions of
workplace behavior in general, by establishing different types of causal
reasoning processes as an important moderating factor in this context.
As a practical matter, our findings are of importance for individuals
who might violate normative stereotypes, by helping them to under-
stand the conditions under which they may or may not be penalized
for such behavior. Moreover, our research has implications for how
managers implement evaluation systems in the workplace, as assess-
ments of employees will depend critically on the intersection of gender
stereotypes and causal reasoning processes.
2|ATTRIBUTIONS UNDER CAUSAL
AMBIGUITY AND TYPES OF CAUSAL
REASONING PROCESSES
In practice, individuals in organizations often cannot clearly attribute
the behavior of other people to one precise reason. Instead, they form
subjective beliefs about the likely causes of another person0s actions
(e.g., Heider, 1958; Kelley, 1967; Martinko, Douglas, & Harvey, 2006;
Weiner, 2008). In general, such causal ambiguity can occur when there
are potential internal as well as external explanations for a behavior or
when there is a general lack of information about likely explanations (e.
g., Ahn, Kalish, Medin, & Gelman, 1995). In these situations of causal
ambiguity, the subjective causal attributions that are made in organiza-
tions have a strong influence on how employees respond to important
issues, such as leader behavior (Martinko & Gardner, 1987), apologies
after acts of workplace deviance (Kim et al., 2006), lowperforming
peers (LePine & Van Dyne, 2001), abusive supervision (Bowling &
Michel, 2011; Breaux, Tepper, Carr, & Folger, 2010; Burton et al.,
2014), new human resource practices (Nishii, Lepak, & Schneider,
2008), or job termination (Barclay, Skarlicki, & Pugh, 2005).
Importantly, even when initial causal ambiguity is later resolved when
more information is available, observers0initial impressions are likely
to have a large influence on how that additional information is proc-
essed and will thus continue to strongly shape individuals0judgments
(e.g., Luchins, 1957; Ross, Lepper, & Hubbard, 1975).
Causal attributions have been classified along various dimensions,
such as locus of control, temporal stability, and general controllability
(e.g., Weiner, 1979). However, prior research (e.g., Heider, 1958;
Kelley & Michela, 1980; Ross & Fletcher, 1985; Trope, 1986) suggests
that the most central dimension of causal attributions is the distinction
between attributions to internal, personal dispositions, such as
personality traits or attitudes, and attributions to external, situational
causes, such as task characteristics or social pressure. Importantly, pre-
vious work on the correspondence bias (for an overview, see, e.g., Gil-
bert & Malone, 1995) has found that observers often fail to consider a
broad range of possible explanations for an observed behavior and
instead make predominantly dispositional attributions. This correspon-
dence bias towards dispositional attributions is frequently driven by a
lack of awareness of possible external explanations and by the ten-
dency of observers to initially draw inferences about the traits of an
observed person and to then (insufficiently) adjust for the possibility
of external explanations (e.g., Gilbert & Malone, 1995).
However, the correspondence bias is strongly mitigated, and
individuals do consider a broader range of possible situational and
dispositional explanations when they engage in a high level of delib-
erative causal reasoning (Blumberg & Silvera, 1998; Gilbert, Pelham,
& Krull, 1988; Gilbert, Krull, & Pelham, 1988). For example, in their
seminal paper on causal reasoning, Gilbert, Pelham, et al. (1988)
argue that once observers fully identify an action taken by a target
person, they go through two steps when making causal attributions.
First, they categorizethat is, they draw dispositional inferences
about the actorand then they correct these inferences by consider-
ing information about possible situational constraints. Importantly,
Gilbert et al. also suggest that Characterization (making dispositional
attributions) is generally a relatively automatic process that requires
little effort or conscious attention, whereas correction (taking into
account possible situational factors) is a more deliberate, relatively
controlled process that uses a significant portion of the perceiver0s
processing resources(p. 734). Consistently, the empirical findings
by Gilbert, Pelham, et al. (1988) showed that observers who
employed only a low level of deliberative causal reasoning made
mostly dispositional attributions. However, when observers invested
sufficient cognitive resources to think more deliberatively about an
observed action, they considered information about possible situa-
tional constraints, and their attributions were both dispositional
and situational, depending on the characteristics of the observed
behavior. This distinction between attributions that are based on
either a high or a low level of deliberative causal reasoning is also
consistent with research on dual process theory, which suggests that
individuals in general make judgments using two different reasoning
types: (a) a default process that is fast, automatic, and effortless or
(b) a process that is slow, deliberative, and effortful and which is
used only when individuals invest sufficient cognitive recourses (for
an overview of dual process theory, see, e.g., Evans & Frankish,
2009). Importantly, these two processes are usually thought of not
as fully mutually exclusive but as a continuum with purely automatic
judgments on one side and purely deliberative judgments on the
other (e.g., Evans & Frankish, 2009).
One important factor that is likely to affect an observer0s level of
deliberative causal reasoning is the salience of causal ambiguity in a
particular situation (Tobin & Weary, 2008; Weary & Jacobson, 1997;
Weary, Vaughn, Stewart, & Edwards, 2006). In particular, if causal
ambiguity is very salient, observers are likely to think more delibera-
tively and thus more carefully about possible internal or external expla-
nations. In contrast, when the salience of causal ambiguity is low,
observers are likely to make mostly dispositional attributions as
KECK AND BABCOCK 277

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT