Uncertainty and Campaigns

Published date01 January 2015
DOI10.1177/1532673X14535075
AuthorDavid A. M. Peterson
Date01 January 2015
Subject MatterArticles
American Politics Research
2015, Vol. 43(1) 109 –143
© The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1532673X14535075
apr.sagepub.com
Article
Uncertainty and
Campaigns: The
Psychological Mechanism
Behind Campaign-
Induced Priming
David A. M. Peterson1
Abstract
Campaigns change how some people vote. How campaigns have this effect
is less well understood. The prevailing view is that these effects occur
by changing the content of voters’ attitudes such as partisanship or issue
positions (persuasion) and by changing the weights voters applied to these
determinants of vote choice (priming). Recent research has challenged
this view and suggests that the support for these priming and persuasion
effects is overstated. Unfortunately, no research directly specifies and tests
the specific psychological mechanism responsible for campaign priming. In
this article, I draw on the differences in the forms of attitude strength and
demonstrate that changes in citizens’ uncertainty are responsible for these
effects. The results suggest that persuasion and changes in uncertainty (but
not ambivalence or importance) are responsible for the changes in voters’
decisions during the campaign. Substantively, the largest effects occur
because of changes in the uncertainty voters have about the nature of the
candidates’ character traits.
Keywords
campaign, priming, attitude strength, uncertainty
1Iowa State University, Ames, USA
Corresponding Author:
David A. M. Peterson, Department of Political Science, Iowa State University, 541 Ross Hall,
Ames, IA 50011, USA.
Email: daveamp@iastate.edu
535075APRXXX10.1177/1532673X14535075American Politics ResearchPeterson
research-article2014
110 American Politics Research 43(1)
Campaigns alter some voters’ decisions (Campbell, 2000; Hillygus &
Jackman, 2003; Holbrook, 1996; Shaw, 1999; Wlezien & Erikson, 2008). If
the campaign did not exist, many voters would make a different choice from
the one they do on Election Day. The long-standing view of presidential elec-
tion campaigns is that they change voters’ minds by persuading and priming
relevant attitudes (Bartels, 2006; Johnston, Blais, Brady, & Crete, 1992;
Johnston, Hagen, & Jamieson, 2004). Priming, in this context is when a cam-
paign focuses on a particular topic and voters’ attitudes about the topic
become more strongly linked to their vote choice, a process Riker (1990)
called heresthetic change. The traditional test of priming is simple: Calculate
the weights applied to an attitude in a vote choice model at one point in time,
and then recalculate the weights applied by the same individuals at a second
moment in time. If the weights increased, the attitude had been primed. For
the past 25 years, the evidence for this seemed incontrovertible.
Recently, Lenz (2009, 2013) has challenged this conventional view of
campaigns by noting that the existing evidence for priming effects are obser-
vationally equivalent with a very different explanation. Instead of the atten-
tion to a topic strengthening the link between that topic and vote choice,
voters may respond to the increased salience by changing their attitudes on
the topic so that they are consistent with their vote choice. Instead of becom-
ing more liberal and, thus, being more likely to vote for the Democratic can-
didate, they become more liberal because they are supporting the Democrat.
Lenz’s results suggest that campaigns do not lead voters to their choices and
that voters do not use elections as opportunities to lead policy.
These are two very different understandings of how campaigns and voters
interact. Under priming theory, voters have preexisting and relatively fixed
attitudes about the topics of the campaign. The information environment
serves to help them decide the weights applied to these considerations. Lenz’s
depiction of voters is less flattering. They form an initial vote choice and shift
their attitude to match this choice. Candidates cannot move voters far and
voters do not respond much to the information they gain. There may be sys-
tematic movement in the underlying attitudes, but the campaigns have mini-
mal influence over voters’ choices.
Lenz’s view of voters’ responsiveness to campaigns may be overly pessi-
mistic, however. Both Lenz’s approach and the priming literature share two
features that may limit their ability to uncover these campaign effects. First,
they both look at single issues in isolation rather than combine a battery of
issue questions into a broader measure, a more common and superior
approach in the issue voting literature (Alvarez, 1997; Alvarez & Nagler,
1998; Ansolabehere, Rodden, & Snyder, 2008; Blais, Turgeon, Gidengil,
Nevitte, & Nadeau, 2004; Treier & Hillygus, 2009). Second, and more impor-
tant, neither Lenz nor the priming studies measure and test a mechanism for
Peterson 111
these changes in the weights applied. They implicitly assume that all voters
should have the same shift in weights and do not measure the psychological
mechanism responsible.1 In this article, I test if changes in a voter’s uncer-
tainty, ambivalence, and importance are responsible for the changing weights
applied to attitudes during a campaign. The results suggest that campaigns
influence voters by helping them become more certain about the candidates.
The article proceeds as follows. In the next section, I develop hypotheses
about how campaigns change the predicted probability of voter’s choice. I
focus on the role of shifts in the content of citizens’ attitudes and the changing
strength of these attitudes. I then move to a discussion of the data, the
Presidential Campaign Impact on Voters: 1976 Panel developed by Patterson
(1976).2 In particular, I address the nature of this panel survey and why it is
the best available data for testing this question. The next section of the article
presents the method for analyzing these data, a nonlinear growth curve. I then
present the results of the statistical models and conclude with what these
results mean for the study of campaigns and how these findings need to be
extended.
Campaigns and Vote Choice: The Problems With
Priming
A voter’s choice in November is a result of three things. First, a voter enters
the campaign with predispositions about politics that provide a starting place
for his or her vote. Their partisanship, issue positions, and other precursors to
vote choice lead them to a baseline decision. For most voters, this baseline is
the way they vote in November (Finkel, 1993; Markus, 1982; Patterson,
1980). The other two portions of a voter’s choice are due to the campaign.
Bartels (2006) and Lenz (2013) suggest that campaigns change a voter’s
decision from this baseline through two processes: persuasion and priming.
Persuasion, or opinion change, is the change in the content of an individual’s
antecedents of vote choice (e.g., issue positions). Priming occurs when the
attitude does not change but the weight that a voter applies to the attitude
changes.
There is ample work believed to document this priming effect in cam-
paigns (Bartels, 2006; Carsey, 2001; Gelman & King, 1993; Johnston et al.,
1992; Johnston et al., 2004). These works share a common design for testing
for this priming effect. They rely on time as the central measure of the cam-
paign, comparing the coefficients from one moment in time to the coeffi-
cients at a later time. If the coefficients increased, the attitude is a stronger
predictor of vote choice and the campaign primed the voter.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT