The Role of Relational Instability on Individual and Partner Outcomes Following Couple Relationship Education Participation

AuthorJennifer Kerpelman,Donna Sollie,Julianne McGill,Scott A. Ketring,Angela B. Bradford,Francesca Adler‐Baeder
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/fare.12201
Published date01 July 2016
Date01 July 2016
J MG  F A-B Auburn University
A B. B Brigham Young University
J K, S A. K,  D S Auburn University
The Role of Relational Instability on Individual and
Partner Outcomes Following Couple Relationship
Education Participation
Some scholars have suggested that distressed
populations may benet more from couple
and relationship education (CRE) than do
their nondistressed counterparts. We examined
this hypothesis using actor–partner interde-
pendence models to explore the relationship
between baseline relational instability and
change for individuals and their partners (379
couples; 758 individuals) who participated in
a CRE program for 6 to 8 weeks. Findings
indicated that a higher level of relational insta-
bility on the part of women was associated with
greater positive change in depressive symptoms.
Furthermore, respondents’ and partners’ base-
line relational instability moderated the change
in women’s couple quality, such that women
reported greater positive change in relationship
quality when reporting higher instability and
higher relationship quality before CRE par-
ticipation, and when their partners reported
higher instability and lower quality before CRE
participation. Men appear to benet from CRE
Department of Human Development and Family Studies,
Auburn University, 203 Spidle Hall, Auburn, AL 36849
(mclanjm@auburn.edu).
Key Words: Actor–partner interdependence model, couple
and relationship education, couple quality,depressive symp-
toms, evaluation methods, family harmony.
participation regardless of baseline relational
instability. Suggestions for researchers and
facilitators are discussed.
B
Some scholars have suggested that relation-
ally unstable couples (i.e., couples considering
divorce or separation) are better served by ther-
apy than by educational forums (e.g., Doherty,
1995). However, a recent model for family life
education and family therapy suggests that these
types of services overlap and that individuals’
needs are best met when the two services work
in conjunction rather than in competition with
each other (Myers-Walls, Ballard, Darling, &
Myers-Bowman, 2011). Regardless, evidence
suggests that couples reporting some level of
instability are attending couple relationship edu-
cation (CRE) programs (Blanchard, Hawkins,
Baldwin, & Fawcett, 2009; DeMaria, 2005; Hal-
ford, O’Donnell, Lizzio, & Wilson, 2006), which
indicates a need to assess outcomes based on
varying levels of relational instability.
Some initial research has emerged nding
benets for CRE participants in less stable rela-
tionships (Bradford et al., 2014; Lucier-Greer,
Adler-Baeder, Harcourt, & Gregson, 2014;
Quirk, Strokoff, Owen, France, & Bergen,
2014). An important next step is to investigate
Family Relations 65 (July 2016): 407–423 407
DOI:10.1111/fare.12201
408 Family Relations
the comparative benets of CRE according to
varying levels of relational instability. In addi-
tion, CRE program evaluation research benets
from exploring changes following CRE partic-
ipation in multiple domains (e.g., individual,
family) using an ecological systems approach.
Further, a dearth of CRE evaluation studies has
assessed dyadic inuences. Thus, our study
contributes to the CRE program evaluation
research by examining dyadic inuences (i.e.,
effects of each partner the other) on multiple
program outcomes for a large, diverse group of
CRE participant couples experiencing different
levels of relational instability.
Theoretical Framework
Most studies of CRE have been atheoreti-
cal, with a few exceptions (e.g., Bradford
et al., 2014; Rauer et al., 2014). To advance
the use of theory in CRE, we utilized a
combination of assumptions from comple-
mentary theories. Our study is framed by the
process-person-context-time assumptions devel-
oped from an ecological systems perspective of
human development (Bronfenbrenner & Morris,
1998; Tudge, Mokrova, Hateld, & Karnik,
2009). We expect that the current and historical
environment is an inuence on individual devel-
opment and assume interactions and linkages
among elements of the individual’s environ-
ment. Along with the contextual inuences
emphasized in the ecological systems perspec-
tive is the expectation for the role individuals
play in their own development by responding to
and interacting with their environment through
processes stimulated by elements of the context
over time (Tudge et al., 2009). In line with this
evolved bioecological approach are assumptions
from the calamity theory of growth (Anthis,
2002; Farson, 1974), which more specically
theorizes that processes during stressful life
experiences—including family-related stressors
(e.g., frequent family conict, indelity, separa-
tion from spouse)—can lead to positive growth
over time.
The calamity theory of growth has been
utilized predominantly in studies of identity
development (e.g., Dalla, Bailey, Cunningham,
Green, & Vyhlidal, 2013; Kunnen, 2006); how-
ever, its use is more recently evident in family
studies as well (e.g., Soulsby & Bennett, 2015).
The theory posits that during stressful life
experiences, such as feeling unsure about the
stability of a committed relationship, a person
becomes focused on the distressful situation
cognitively and emotionally. This attention
may facilitate more help-seeking behaviors and
receptivity to learning and implementing new
skills, thus resulting in positive growth. In other
words, individuals may be more receptive to
initiating positive changes during trying times.
For example, Anthis (2002) found that stressful
life experiences (e.g., death of a loved one, fam-
ily nancial concerns) lead to more exploration
and change over time. A recent study (Soulsby
& Bennett, 2015) more specically focused
on stress in relationships (e.g., transition to
marriage, cohabitation, and divorce) and found
that these transitional periods, whether viewed
as positive or negative, positively affected each
individual’s personal growth and self-concept.
This phenomenon has been documented over
a 5-month time frame without intervention
(Anthis, 2002). Thus, we expect that experi-
encing a brief intervention in the context of
a stressful event such as recent or historical
relational instability will result in incremental
but measurable positive changes. Further, these
changes may be comparatively greater than
those experienced by less relationally distressed
participants in CRE.
Also relevant to our study, a bioecological
systems framework assumes that couple func-
tioning is linked to individual functioning, as
well as overall family functioning. In addition,
it is assumed that microsystems or subsystems
(e.g., the family as a whole, the couple rela-
tionship) create a shared context within which
an individual’s, as well as his or her partner’s,
outcomes are inuenced over time (Halford &
Wilson, 2009; Whitchurch & Constantine,
1993), thereby prescribing the empirical con-
sideration and assessment of cross-dyad effects.
This theoretical framework guides the present
study of changes following CRE participation.
Couple and Relationship Education
Extensive research has been conducted over
several decades on the impact of couple
and relationship education programs that
target the promotion of healthy relation-
ship skills (Administration for Children
and Families, 2007; Blanchard et al., 2009;
Carroll & Doherty, 2003; Hawkins, Blan-
chard, Baldwin, & Fawcett, 2008). Several

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT