THE PRAGMATIC AMERICAN: EMPIRICAL REALITY OR METHODOLOGICAL ARTIFACT?

Date01 May 2014
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/1745-9125.12035
Published date01 May 2014
THE PRAGMATIC AMERICAN: EMPIRICAL REALITY
OR METHODOLOGICAL ARTIFACT?
JUSTIN T. PICKETT1and THOMAS BAKER2
1School of Criminal Justice, University at Albany, SUNY
2L. Douglas Wilder School of Government and Public Affairs, Virginia
Commonwealth University
KEYWORDS: public opinion, response acquiescence, crime attributions, punitiveness,
rehabilitation
Scholars widely agree that the public is pragmatic about criminal justice. The empir-
ical basis for this conclusion is the failure in several previous studies to find a sizable
negative relationship between dispositional and situational crime attributions, or be-
tween support for punitive and rehabilitative crime policies. We suggest, however, that
public pragmatism may be an artifact of the use of unidirectional question batteries in
prior research to measure attribution styles and policy support. When such questions
are used, acquiescent responding can introduce systematic error that is positively cor-
related across items and scales. Drawing on data from an experiment with a national
sample (N =826) of Internet panelists, we examine how this methodological approach
impacts the bivariate correlations and multivariate relationships between attribution
styles and between support for punitive and rehabilitative crime policies. The findings
reveal that using unidirectional sets of questions to measure these concepts likely re-
sults in 1) inflated alpha reliability coefficients, 2) an underestimation of the magnitude
of the negative relationships between attribution styles and between punitiveness and
support for rehabilitation, and 3) an underestimation of the extent to which punitive-
ness and support for rehabilitation are driven by the same factors, working in opposite
directions.
A substantial and growing body of research has investigated public opinion about
the causes of crime and the appropriate criminal justice response to offending (Ap-
plegate, Cullen, and Fisher, 1997; Applegate et al., 2000; Cochran, Boots, and Heide,
2003; Gabbidon and Boisvert, 2012; Grasmick and McGill, 1994; Pickett and Chiri-
cos, 2012; Ramirez, 2013; Thompson and Bobo, 2011; Tyler and Boeckmann, 1997;
Unnever and Cullen, 2010). This line of inquiry has been particularly salient be-
cause of the documented influence of popular attitudes on criminal justice policy and
court decisions (Baumer and Martin, 2013; Brace and Boyea, 2008; Enns, in press;
Additional supporting information can be found in the listing for this article in the Wiley Online
Library at http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/crim.2011.52.issue-2/issuetoc.
Direct correspondence to Justin T. Pickett, School of Criminal Justice, University at Albany,
SUNY, 135 Western Avenue, Albany, NY 12222 (e-mail: jpickett@albany.edu). This work was
funded by the University at Albany Faculty Research Awards Program (FRAP)-Category A. The
authors are grateful to Shawn Bushway for his assistance with collecting the data, and to Christina
Mancini and Christi Metcalfe for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript.
C2014 American Society of Criminology doi: 10.1111/1745-9125.12035
CRIMINOLOGY Volume 52 Number 2 195–222 2014 195
196 PICKETT & BAKER
Nicholson-Crotty, Peterson, and Ramirez, 2009). An important finding has been that
members of the public “are profoundly pragmatic when they consider the issue of crime
and its control” (Unnever et al., 2010: 453); they attribute crime to both individual failings
(dispositional attributions) and environmental influences (situational attributions) and,
as a result, simultaneously support punishment and rehabilitation (McCorkle, 1993;
Pickett, Chiricos, and Gertz, 2014). Thus, in their seminal review of the research, Cullen,
Fisher, and Applegate (2002: 60) concluded that “the central tendency in public opinion
is to be punitive and progressive.” Likewise, Mascini and Houtman (2006: 832) have
argued that “the habit of conceiving of support for repression and for rehabilitation as
diametrically opposed options should have been abandoned long ago.”
The existence of public pragmatism has strong policy implications. Political respon-
siveness to the interests of a pragmatic public would involve bipartisan efforts to develop
a multifaceted and balanced approach to crime control that ensures tough punishments
for offenders and provides them with rehabilitative programming. The theoretical impli-
cations of public pragmatism are also significant. Evidence of pragmatism suggests that
in the popular view, punitiveness and support for rehabilitation are distinct attitudinal
phenomena, rather than opposing sides of a single construct (Unnever et al., 2010). If
this is accurate, and punitiveness and support for rehabilitation are not opposites, then
they should not simply be related in opposite ways to the same key factors (Mascini and
Houtman, 2006; McCorkle, 1993). A central tenet of the pragmatism thesis, then, is that
the two types of policy support are best explained by separate theoretical models (Mascini
and Houtman, 2006; Pickett, Mancini, and Mears, 2013).
The concern raised in this article, however, is that the pragmatism identified in prior
studies may be an artifact of the methodology typically employed in public opinion re-
search in the field of criminology and criminal justice. Despite the overwhelming ev-
idence of acquiescence in survey responding (Krosnick, 1999; Saris et al., 2010; Schu-
man and Presser, 1981), researchers often have constructed measures of key concepts—
crime attributions, punitiveness, and support for rehabilitation—from batteries of ques-
tions that are unilaterally positive, where a response of “agree,” “support,” or “yes” in-
dicates the presence of the concept of interest.1In these instances, acquiescence—“the
tendency to answer items in a positive way regardless of their content” (Billiet and
Davidov, 2008: 543)—can result in systematic measurement error that biases observed
scores and relationships by introducing nonsubstantive variation in observations that is
positively correlated across items and scales (Baumgartner and Steenkamp, 2001). This
finding is important because the strongest evidence of pragmatism has been the absence
of a hydraulic relation—a sizeable negative correlation—between either dispositional and
situational attributions, or punitiveness and support for rehabilitation (Mascini and Hout-
man, 2006; Unnever et al., 2010). Indeed, the absence of this hypothesized negative cor-
relation is the criterion for pragmatism that Unnever et al. (2010: 433) identified at the
outset of their analysis: “[T]he failure to detect a hydraulic relation would suggest that
Americans are more centrist and pragmatic in their views on crime and its control.”
In the current study, we evaluate how acquiescence bias may have influenced both the
quality of the measures used in prior research and the resultant findings about public
1. The problems associated with response acquiescence are not limited to agree/disagree items; they
emerge across many different types of questions when unidirectional sets of statements are used
(Blamey, Bennet, and Morrison, 1999; Krosnick, 1999; Schuman and Presser, 1981).

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT