The Impact of Neuroimages in the Sentencing Phase of Capital Trials

AuthorKent A. Kiehl,Eyal Aharoni,N. J. Schweitzer,Michael J. Saks
Published date01 March 2014
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/jels.12036
Date01 March 2014
The Impact of Neuroimages in the
Sentencing Phase of Capital Trials
Michael J. Saks, N. J. Schweitzer, Eyal Aharoni, and Kent A. Kiehl*
Although recent research has found that neurological expert testimony is more persuasive
than other kinds of expert and nonexpert evidence, no impact has been found for
neuroimages beyond that of neurological evidence sans images. Those findings hold true in
the context of a mens rea defense and various forms of insanity defenses. The present studies
test whether neuroimages afford heightened impact in the penalty phase of capital murder
trials. Two mock jury experiments (n=825 and n=882) were conducted online using
nationally representative samples of persons who were jury eligible and death qualified.
Participants were randomly assigned to experimental conditions varying the defendant’s
diagnosis (psychopathy, schizophrenia, normal), type of expert evidence supporting the
diagnosis (clinical, genetic, neurological sans images, neurological with images), evidence of
future dangerousness (high, low), and whether the proponent of the expert evidence was
the prosecution (arguing aggravation) or the defense (arguing mitigation). For defendants
diagnosed as psychopathic, neuroimages reduced judgments of responsibility and sentences
of death. For defendants diagnosed as schizophrenic, neuroimages increased judgments of
responsibility; nonimage neurological evidence decreased death sentences and judgments
of responsibility and dangerousness. All else equal, psychopaths were more likely to be
sentenced to death than schizophrenics. When experts opined that the defendant was
dangerous, sentences of death increased. A backfire effect was found such that the offering
party produced the opposite result than that being argued for when the expert evidence was
clinical, genetic, or nonimage neurological, but when the expert evidence included
neuroimages, jurors moved in the direction argued by counsel.
I. Introduction
A. Expectations of Inordinate Effects of Neuroimages
In criminal trials, brain images are among the most recent forms of evidence presented for
the purpose of supporting factual arguments about the defendant’s mental condition. That
images would enhance persuasion should not be surprising when we consider the sizeable
*Address correspondence to Michael Saks, Sandra Day O’Connor College of Law, Arizona State University, Box
877906, Tempe, AZ 85287-7906, email: saks@asu.edu. Saks is Regents Professor in the College of Law and Department
of Psychology; Schweitzer is Assistant Professor of Social and Behavioral Sciences, New College of Interdisciplinary
Arts and Sciences, Arizona State University; Aharoni was Postdoctoral Fellow at the University of New Mexico and is
now Associate Behavioral Scientist, RAND Corporation; Kiehl is Associate Professor of Psychology and Neuroscience,
University of New Mexico.
This research was supported by a grant from the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation to the Law and
Neuroscience Project.
bs_bs_banner
Journal of Empirical Legal Studies
Volume 11, Issue 1, 105–131, March 2014
105
body of research finding that images enhance learning compared to textual-only commu-
nication in educational settings (Carney & Levin 2002; Mayer 2009).
Numerous commentators have expressed concern that these images are inordinately
persuasive—far more so than the life history of the defendant, or the interview-based
diagnoses of a clinical psychologist or psychiatrist, or the tests of a neuropsychologist, or
even a radiologist’s verbal description of the findings of an MRI or fMRI. The notion that
has been advanced is that seeing the image of a brain with their own eyes leads judges and
jurors to believe something that they will be unable to deny: that the suspect’s brain appears
different, that the observed difference is beyond the control of the defendant, and—at the
end of this line of reasoning or intuition—that the extreme behavior that brought the
defendant to court is not something for which he or she can be held responsible. In this
view, if the brain is the source of behavior, then a defective brain must be the cause of the
deviant behavior. The image, and only the image, it has been suggested, can lead judges
and jurors to this unshakable conclusion (e.g., Batts 2009; Brown & Murphy 2010; Compton
2010; Dumit 1999; Gurley & Marcus 2008; Khoshbin & Khoshbin 2007; Kulynych 1997; Pratt
2005; Rose 2000; Roskies 2007, 2008).
If brain images have far more influence on factfinders than is justified by the
information those images convey, then the courts will be asked to exclude such images.
Rule 403 of the Federal Rules of Evidence, and similar or identical rules in all states,
provides that otherwise relevant evidence “may be excluded if its probative value is sub-
stantially outweighed by the danger of unfair prejudice, confusion of the issues, or mislead-
ing the jury . . . .” Courts prefer admission of relevant evidence, so exclusion on these
grounds is unusual, but if neuroimages were found to routinely win the acquiescence of
jurors despite adding little or nothing to the information provided by a witness using other
means to explain the findings of an examination of a defendant or, indeed, if the
neuroimages somehow turned the tide in circumstances where evidence in other forms was
not terribly persuasive, then the images would be serious candidates for exclusion.
The neurosciences are revealing growing evidence for neural markers of poor
behavioral control (Aharoni et al. in press) and biomarkers for mental illnesses like schizo-
phrenia (Du et al. 2012) and psychopathy (Ermer et al. 2012, 2013), but it is not the case
that just any neural cause of antisocial behavior is sufficient to legally excuse that behavior.
The ongoing challenge for neuroscientists and lawyers is to reliably distinguish legally
excusable neural causes from inexcusable ones, and to present this information in an
unbiased manner. To date, there is no clear consensus on what criteria ought to be used in
the performance of such tasks.
Moreover, brain images created from techniques such as functional MRI (fMRI) are
the product of a complex set of choices and manipulations: the scanner, its programming,
its measures, the tasks selected for testing patients and subjects, the characteristics of the
sampled participants, the analyses carried out on the resulting time-series data, and the
soundness of the statistics used in the analysis. Interpretation of what constitute abnormal
patterns might require comparison to a normative database. Unfortunately, such normative
databases are rare. Further, many brain functions, and certainly brain structure, change
over time, so a test today may not necessarily tell us anything about acute brain function
(i.e., a psychotic event) at the time of the conduct at issue in a criminal case. In short, no
106 Saks et al.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT