The Impact of Complex Family Structure on Child Well‐being: Evidence From Siblings

Published date01 August 2018
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12456
AuthorTarek Mostafa,Heather Joshi,Ludovica Gambaro
Date01 August 2018
T M University College London Institute of Education
L G Deutsches Institut für Wirtschaftsforschung (German Institute for
Economic Research) Berlin
H J University College London Institute of Education∗∗
The Impact of Complex Family Structure on Child
Well-being: Evidence From Siblings
Evidence from the United Kingdom Millennium
Cohort on children at ages 3 and 5 with older
siblings addresses the questions of whether
those living with both biological parents and
only full siblings have better emotional and
behavior outcomes than other children, and
whether nonfull siblings affect children’s out-
comes independently of parents’ partnership
status. Adjusting for measured family cir-
cumstances and resources in cross-sectional
regressions accounted for much of the adverse
Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Department of Social
Sciences, University College London Institute of
Education, 20 Bedford Way,London WC1H 0AL, UK
(t.mostafa@ucl.ac.uk).
Deutsches Institut für Wirtschaftsforschung (German
Institute for Economic Research) Berlin, Mohrenstraße 58,
10117 Berlin, Germany.
∗∗Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Department of Social
Sciences, University College London Institute of
Education, 20 Bedford Way,London WC1H 0AL, UK.
© 2018 The Authors. Journal of Marriage and Familypub-
lished by WileyPeriodicals, Inc. on behalf of National Coun-
cil on Family Relations.
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribu-
tion and reproduction in any medium, provided the original
work is properly cited.
Key Words: child well-being, family structure, longitudinal
research, siblings, stepfamilies.
association of family complexity with child out-
comes. Controlling for unobserved family and
child xed effects did not, however, attenuate all
estimates further. Fixed unobservable factors
appeared to be masking underlying associa-
tions. Allowing for them intensied some, albeit
modest, estimates. These revealed excess exter-
nalizing behavior problems for boys with single
or stepparents but only full siblings. For girls
with single mothers, the chances of internalizing
problems were raised. Whether siblings were
full or not made little difference to outcomes in
general.
In recent decades, patterns of partnership and
parenthood have changed profoundly in many
industrialized countries, with implications for
the living arrangements in which children are
brought up (Lesthaeghe, 1995; McLanahan,
2004). Children living with both biological par-
ents tend to have better developmental outcomes
than children who do not. This empirical regu-
larity is found in the United States (Carlson &
Corcoran, 2001; Cooksey, 1997; McLanahan &
Sandefur, 1994) as well as other industrialized
countries (Björklund, Ginther, & Sundström,
2007; Ermisch & Francesconi, 2001). As family
forms have diversied,the literature has not only
contrasted the experiences of children in one-
and two-adult families but also distinguished
between married and cohabiting couples and the
902 Journal of Marriage and Family 80 (August 2018): 902–918
DOI:10.1111/jomf.12456
Complex Families: Evidence From Sibling Data 903
formation of stepfamilies (Artis, 2007; Biblarz
& Raftery, 1999; Brown, 2004; Case, Lin, &
McLanahan, 2001; Hofferth, 2006; Hofferth &
Anderson, 2003; Sweeney, 2010). Increasing
partnership instability and multipartner fertility
mean that children are less likely to live with
both biological parents and are also more likely
to be brought up sharing a home with half- or
stepsiblings (Brown, Stykes, & Manning, 2016;
Carlson & Furstenberg, 2006; Guzzo, 2014). Yet
family structure research has focused more on
parent–child relationships, paying less attention
to the conguration of sibling relationships in
the family.
Siblings form another dimension to a child’s
experience of family life, which is likely to
have implications for child development (Brody,
1998; Dunn, 2005). Siblings can support each
other during stressful events or be an additional
source of maladjustment. Although psycho-
logical literature on the quality of siblings’
relationships downplays the importance of
biological relatedness (Gass, Jenkins, & Dunn,
2007), there is emerging evidence from family
structure research showing that children who
live with nonfull siblings fare worse than those
who do not (Halpern-Meekin & Tach, 2008;
Tillman, 2008).
In this article, we suggest that the relationship
between family structure and child development
is best understood when families are studied
holistically and sibling constellations are taken
into account alongside parent–child relation-
ships. Drawing on research on stepfamilies
and on the few studies distinguishing nontra-
ditional siblings (Fomby, Goode, & Mollborn,
2016; Gennetian, 2005; Halpern-Meekin &
Tach, 2008; Tillman, 2008), we distinguish
between biological two-parent, stepparent,
and single-parent families as well as families
with full and nonfull siblings. This provides a
more complete picture of the family structure
than usually made in the literature. We use the
term family complexity to indicate this way of
conceptualizing children’s living arrangement,
which integrates the traditional approach based
on child–parent relationships with information
on children’s relationships to their coresiding
siblings (Brown, Manning, & Stykes, 2015). Our
contribution is based on previously unanalyzed
evidence about children’s behavioraladjustment
in families with more than one sibling in the
United Kingdom, where data of the appropriate
structure are relatively rare.
W F C M M
There are only a few studies, all from the
United States, that expand the traditional family
structure classication to investigate the impact
of living with half- or stepsiblings, but this
emerging line of research points to adverse
effects for children and youth. Adolescents
living with half- or stepsiblings were found to
have poorer academic achievement and higher
levels of depression, school-related behavioral
difculties, and delinquency than children liv-
ing with full siblings only (Halpern-Meekin
& Tach, 2008; Tillman, 2008). These associ-
ations cut across different parental structures
and remained after controlling for family back-
ground characteristics, family instability, quality
of interpersonal relationships within the family,
and parental investment in children. Moreover,
the negative association of family complexity
with youth academic achievement was much
stronger for males than females (Tillman, 2008),
in line with the nding that girls forge more pos-
itive relationships with their siblings of any sort
than do boys (Anderson & Rice, 1992). Ginther
and Pollak (2004) found little difference in
the educational outcomes of joint children and
stepchildren from the same complex families,
but both groups of children had worse academic
outcomes than children who were residing with
full siblings only. Similarly, Gennetian (2005)
reported the possibility of a small negative
effect of living with half- and stepsiblings on the
cognitive scores of children aged 5 to 10. The
results on children’s emotional and behavioral
adjustment are similar. Fomby et al. (2016)
showed that among kindergarten children, those
living with half- or stepsiblings displayed worse
behavioral scores when compared with their
peers with full siblings only and whose parents
had the same union status, even after extensive
controls on parental nancial, material, and
emotional resources. Hofferth (2006) also found
more emotional and behavioral difculties
among children aged 3 to 12 years with complex
sibships.
There are a number of explanations of why
these negative associations might be effects
of family complexities on child development,
which broadly draw on theories about stress
and parental investment (Tillman, 2008). Stress
theory asserts that major or highly disruptive
events create strain and psychological distress
(Amato, 2000; Fomby & Cherlin, 2007; Thoits,
1995). Changes in parental partnership may put

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT