The Effect of Blinded Experts on Juror Verdicts

DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/j.1740-1461.2012.01273.x
AuthorDavid V. Yokum,Christopher T. Robertson
Date01 December 2012
Published date01 December 2012
The Effect of Blinded Experts on
Juror Verdicts
Christopher T. Robertson and David V. Yokum*
“Blind expertise” has been proposed as an institutional solution to the problem of bias in
expert witness testimony in litigation (Robertson 2010). At the request of a litigant, an
intermediary selects a qualified expert and pays the expert to review a case without knowing
which side requested the opinion. This article reports an experiment that tests the hypoth-
esis that, compared to traditional experts, such “blinded experts” will be more persuasive to
jurors. A national sample of mock jurors (N=275) watched an online video of a staged
medical malpractice trial, including testimony from two medical experts, one of whom (or
neither, in the control condition) was randomly assigned to be a blind expert. We also
manipulated whether the judge provided a special jury instruction explaining the blinding
concept. Descriptively, the data suggest juror reluctance to impose liability. Despite an
experimental design that included negligent medical care, only 46 percent of the jurors
found negligence in the control condition, which represents the status quo. Blind experts,
testifying on either side, were perceived as significantly more credible, and were more
highly persuasive, in that they doubled (or halved) the odds of a favorable verdict, and
increased (or decreased) simulated damages awards by over $100,000. The increased
damages award appears to be due to jurors hedging their damages awards, which interacted
with the blind expert as a driver of certainty. Use of a blind expert may be a rational strategy
for litigants, even without judicial intervention in the form of special jury instructions or
otherwise.
I. Background
The U.S. legal system tasks judges and jurors—both laypersons as to science—with resolving
highly technical questions. These laypersons are asked, for example, to evaluate DNA
evidence to determine whether it inculpates a particular defendant, to determine the
standard of care for lumbar radiculopathy, to interpret epidemiological data to determine
whether a given chemical causes an observed disease, and to ascertain the state of the art in
a patent suit for computer software. Thus, in both civil and criminal litigation, expert
*Address correspondence to Christopher T. Robertson, James E. Rogers College of Law, the University of Arizona, PO
Box 210176, Tucson, AZ 85721; email: chris.robertson@law.arizona.edu. Yokum is J.D./Ph.D. candidate, James E.
Rogers College of Law, Department of Psychology, The University of Arizona.
Thanks to Gregory Schwartz, Aaron Kesselheim, and Tom Mauet for serving as actors and consultants; to James
Greiner, George Kimball Smith, and anonymous reviewers for the Conference on Empirical Legal Studies and Journal
of Empirical Legal Studies for commenting on drafts, and to Germar Townsend, Tess Gemberling, Carol Ward, Judy
Parker, Barbara Lopez, and Bert Sky for excellent research and administrative support.
bs_bs_banner
Journal of Empirical Legal Studies
Volume 9, Issue 4, 765–794, December 2012
765
witnesses play an increasingly prominent and important role to inform those layperson
decisionmakers. In civil litigation, experts appear in the vast majority of trials (Gross 1991),
and courts have held in the criminal context that the right to effective counsel includes the
right to funding for expert testimony (Ake v. Oklahoma).
In the U.S. legal system, expert witnesses are hand-picked by the litigants, coached
toward favorable opinions, and compensated for continuing work only as long as those
opinions are favorable (Robertson 2010). These experts are thus subject to various cog-
nitive biases, which have been extensively documented in the social sciences (Risinger
et al. 2002). Reflecting on his own experience, John Langbein (1985) has explained that
“those of us who serve as expert witnesses are known as ‘saxophones[,]’ . . . a musical
instrument on which the lawyer sounds the desired notes.” Courts have criticized experts
who altogether “cast aside [their] scholar’s mantle and [became] a shill” for the party
that retained them (Mid-State Fertilizer Co. v. Exchange National Bank of Chicago). The
problem is not limited to outliers, however. In one study, both federal judges and attor-
neys cited “experts abandon[ing] objectivity and becom[ing] advocates for the side
that hired them” as the most frequent problem with expert testimony (Johnson et al.
2000).
Experimental studies have shown that witnesses interviewed by partisan attorneys
prior to testifying tend to deliver more biased testimony than those interviewed by nonpar-
tisan attorneys (Sheppard & Vidmar 1980). In criminal cases, scholars have documented a
significant bias of expert opinions in the direction of the side that requested the opinion
(Otto 1989). One litigation-oriented study (Gitlin et al. 2004) compared the opinions
rendered by radiologists retained by plaintiffs’ attorneys in asbestos cases to opinions
rendered by a panel of “independent” radiologists retained by defense attorneys.1The
plaintiffs’ experts found physiological abnormalities 95.9 percent of the time, while “inde-
pendent” reviewers found abnormalities only 4.5 percent of the time. These sorts of biases
may operate subconsciously (e.g., an anchoring heuristic driven by the litigant’s suggestion
as to the amount of damages) or consciously (e.g., a desire to maintain a stream of income
from the litigant).
Expert biases may be the subject of cross-examination in trials, but that is typically a
zero-sum game, since both litigants tend to exploit these biases. Although court appoint-
ment of experts is a possible solution, it is almost never actually employed, for cultural and
economic reasons, in the adversarial U.S. legal system (Gross 1991; Cecil & Willging 1993).
Without a real alternative to biased expert witnesses, it is not practicable for judges to simply
exclude all such testimony; instead, the exclusion doctrine—based on Daubert v. Merrell Dow
Pharmaceuticals—operates only in marginal cases. It is not a solution for the everyday
instances of litigant-induced biases.
When layperson factfinders (whether judge or jury) are left to rely on traditional
expert witnesses who are subject to these biases, the factfinders receive relatively weak
1We put “independent” in scare quotes because the study authors admit that the research was done on behalf of
defense attorneys. See Oliver et al. (2004), arguing that the Gitlin “study was done at the behest of attorneys for
defendants in asbestos litigation.”
766 Robertson and Yokum

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT