Taking sides: Gender and third‐party partisanship in disputes

AuthorAndrew Krajewski,Ethan M. Rogers,Mark T. Berg,Richard B. Felson
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/1745-9125.12215
Date01 November 2019
Published date01 November 2019
Received: 8 October 2018 Revised: 29 April 2019 Accepted: 1 May 2019
DOI: 10.1111/1745-9125.12215
ARTICLE
Taking sides: Gender and third-party partisanship
in disputes
Ethan M. Rogers1Richard B. Felson2Mark T. Berg1
Andrew Krajewski2
1Department of Sociology, University of Iowa
Public Policy Center, University of Iowa
2Department of Sociology and Criminology,
Pennsylvania State University
Correspondence
Ethan M. Rogers,Public Policy Center, Uni-
versityof Iowa, 207 South Quadrangle, 310 S.
GrandAvenue, Iowa City,IA 52242.
Email:ethan-rogers@uiowa.edu
Fundinginformation
NationalInstitute of Justice, Grant/Award
Number:#2012-91288-PA-IJ
Theaut horswould like to thank the anony-
mousreviewers and Janet Lauritsen for their
constructivefeedback during the review
process.
Abstract
We examine the role of a norm protecting womenin under-
standing third-party partisanship in verbal and violent dis-
putes. Our analyses are based on reports provided by male
inmates and men they know who have never been arrested.
The results show that third parties are more likely to sup-
port female adversaries than male adversaries. The gender
effect is stronger when we control for the relational dis-
tance between adversaries, which indicates that a privacy
norm might inhibit this normative protection. The gender
effect is somewhat weaker when we control for the relative
physical size of the adversaries, which indicates that a
general norm protecting vulnerable people partly explains
the gender effect. The strong gender effect that remains,
however, demonstrates the importance of the normative
protection of women, regardless of relative size, during
disputes. The results have implications for research on
situational factors in violence and violence against women.
KEYWORDS
conflict, gender, partisanship, third parties, violence
Whether a dispute culminates in physical violence depends, in part, on social norms. Some scholars
have argued that when the dispute is between a man and a woman, a norm protecting women (NPW)
inhibits the man from attacking the woman and motivates third parties to protect her (Felson, 2002;
Pinker, 2011; Straus, 2006). Researchers suggest this norm might explain why men commit less
physical and verbal aggression against women than against other men (Archer, 2000, 2006; Cross &
Campbell, 2011, 2012; Cross, Tee, & Campbell, 2011). Support for the NPW’s effects on third parties
comes from a broad body of research findings indicating that third parties are more likely to condemn
Criminology. 2019;57:579–602. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/crim © 2019 American Society of Criminology 579
580 ROGERS ET AL.
male aggression against women than male aggression against men. Evidence reviewed in this article
shows that third parties evaluate it more negatively, punish it more harshly, and report it to legal
authorities more frequently. The effectof gender on third-par ty partisanship during disputes, however,
has never been studied. In addition, researchers have not studied why gender influences partisanship
or other third-party involvement in disputes. Understanding the nature of these effects is important in
the study of gender violence and dispute escalation.
Partisans refer to third parties who provide support to one adversary in a dispute over another by
acting as informers, advisers, advocates, allies, or surrogates (Black & Baumgartner, 1983). They can
influence whether altercations become violent, whether an adversary is injured, and whether one adver-
sary prevails over the other (Cooney, 1998; Felson & Steadman, 1983; Pruitt & Rubin, 1986). Partisans
may play a particularly important role in intergender disputes if the NPW leads them to intervene on
behalf of women.
Social norms, however, vary in their degree of application and enforcement (Morris, 1956). When
witnessing interpersonal disputes, third parties may not protect women when they think it might be
costly or they do not feel personallyresponsible for handling the situation (Schwartz, 1968). They may
find it dangerous to support women during their altercations with men, particularly when those men are
violent offenders. High costs have been shown toi nterfere with the tendency to protect women during
disasters. For instance, evidence suggests that men are more likely to survive maritime disasters than
women in spite of the NPW (Elinder & Erixson, 2012). Apparently, the tendency of men to prioritize
women’s access to lifeboats on the Titanic was an exception to this pattern (Frey, Savage, & Torgler,
2011; Hall, 1986).
In addition, scholars havenot sufficiently evaluated whether other conflict-related norms have impli-
cations for the association between gender and third-party partisanship. First, the impact of the NPW
may be offset by the norm of privacy. A privacy norm may inhibit partisanship on behalf of women
during intergender disputes because disputes between men and women tend to involve people in close
relationships. Second, a vulnerability norm, rather than the NPW, may explain why third parties are
more supportive of women. It may be that support for women reflects a tendency to protect persons
who are perceived as more physically vulnerable. This norm is not exclusive to women but applies to
anyone who is perceived to have a weak coercive capacity (Pruitt & Rubin, 1986). Scholars have not
examined the vulnerability norm as an alternative explanation of gender effects on partisanship.
In this research, we assess whether the gender of adversaries is associated with third-party
partisanship in violent and nonviolent disputes reported by men. We base our analyses on a survey of
recently admitted male prison inmates and their male contacts in the community who had never been
arrested. Each respondent was asked to describe a verbal and a physically violent dispute that they
had been involved in while living in the community. These data contain information on the situational
characteristics of disputes that is not available in most other studies (cf. Phillips & Cooney, 2005).
Moreover, our use of a sample of inmates and their community contacts, rather than a sample of the
general population, allows for us to obtain information on a sufficient number of violent incidents
(see Lauritsen & Laub, 2007).
We examine three issues in this research. First, we investigate whether third parties are more likely
to support female adversaries than male adversaries. Second, we examine whether the effect of the
adversary’s gender on third-party support for adversaries increases when we control for the relational
distance between adversaries. A suppressor effect would indicate that a privacy norm offsets the
effects of the NPW. Finally, we test whether the effect of the adversary’s gender decreases when we
control for the relative physical size of adversaries. Evidence of mediation would mean that gender
effects are a result of a vulnerability norm, in which people intervene on behalf of physically weaker
parties.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT