Subjective time in organizations: Conceptual clarification, integration, and implications for future research

AuthorAndreas W. Richter,Shi Tang,Sucheta Nadkarni
Published date01 February 2020
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1002/job.2421
Date01 February 2020
THE JOB ANNUAL REVIEW AND CONCEPTUAL DEVELOPMENT
ISSUE
Subjective time in organizations: Conceptual clarification,
integration, and implications for future research
Shi Tang |Andreas W. Richter |Sucheta Nadkarni
Cambridge Judge Business School, University
of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
Correspondence
Shi Tang, Cambridge Judge Business School,
University of Cambridge, The Old Schools,
Trinity Ln, Cambridge CB2 1TN, United
Kingdom.
Email: st637@cam.ac.uk
Shortly after completing her work on this
manuscript, Sucheta sadly passed away. We
would like to dedicate this paper to the
inspiration, enthusiasm, and dedication that
Sucheta has given to us, and to the research on
temporal phenomena in organizations more
broadly.
Summary
Despite the rapid growth of organizational research on subjective time, the extant lit-
erature is fragmented due to a lack of conceptual clarification and integration of tem-
poral constructs. To address this fragmentation, we synthesize temporal research
from both organizational behavior and adjacent disciplines (i.e., strategy, entrepre-
neurship, and organizational theory) and introduce a framework that allocates tempo-
ral constructs according to their basic conceptual nature (traitstate) and level of
analysis (individualcollective). We employed the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count
text analysis to determine the traitstate property of the constructs and a coding
method to determine their level of analysis. This framework categorizes four generic
types of subjective time: individual temporal disposition,individual temporal state,
collective temporal state, and collective temporal disposition. We clarify the conceptual-
izations of the temporal constructs belonging to each of the four archetypes of sub-
jective time and review their key findings in the organizational literature. Based on
this integrative framework, we identify critical knowledge gaps in the current state
of research and chart a future agenda with specific suggestions.
KEYWORDS
multilevel research, subjective time, temporal constructs, traitstate distinction
1|INTRODUCTION
Time plays a central role in organizational life (Bluedorn & Denhardt,
1988; Shipp & Cole, 2015). But it does not merely exist objectively
as clock time. It is also experienced by organizational actors
1
in subjec-
tive ways, as indicated by expressions such as time flies!or we are
pressed for time!Objective time (i.e., clock time) is linear (progressing
in a unidirectional way), homogeneous (the passage of each unit is the
same), and uniform (identical across individuals and situations); in
contrast, subjective time manifests in richer forms that are nonlinear,
heterogeneous, and multiform (Ancona et al., 2001; Shipp & Cole,
2015; Shipp & Fried, 2014).
Organizational actors' subjective experience of time varies accord-
ing to their innate traits, ingrained beliefs, and influences of external
situations or events (Bluedorn, 2002; McGrath & Kelly, 1986; Shipp
& Fried, 2014). Recent decades have seen a proliferation of organiza-
tional behavior (OB) research examining a variety of temporal con-
structs that capture how time is subjectively valued, understood,
used, or perceived in organizational contexts (Kooij, Kanfer, Betts, &
Rudolph, 2018; Shipp & Cole, 2015; Shipp, Edwards, & Lambert,
2009). Despite this notable growth in research on subjective time,
the literature remains somewhat fragmented and would benefit from
conceptual clarification and integration on at least two fronts.
First, conceptual ambiguity caused by jinglejanglefallacies (Block,
1995) pervades in the conceptualizations of many temporal constructs.
Jingle fallacy occurs when the same label is used to describe different
phenomena. Take future orientation as an exampleDas and Teng
1
Following Ancona, Okhuysen, and Perlow (2001), we use the term organizational actorto
refer to the organizational unit across multiple levels of analysisfrom individuals, to teams,
to organizations.
Received: 14 November 2017 Revised: 17 September 2019 Accepted: 15 October 2019
DOI: 10.1002/job.2421
© 2019 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/job J Organ Behav. 2020;41:210234.
210
(1998) used this term to denote the length of time horizon underlying
one's consideration of the future, whereas Balliet and Ferris (2013) used
the same term to capture one's attentional focus on the future time
frame. Moreover, in the same study, Balliet and Ferris (2013, p. 300)
conceptualized future orientation as both individual differences in
future orientation(measured on a Likert scale) and futureoriented
states(induced in experiments). However, these two phenomena are
grounded in fundamentally distinct theoretical perspectivesthe for-
mer characterizes a dispositional tendency (i.e., trait), whereas the latter
features a malleable, situationelicited state. These jingle fallacies can
lead to severe confusions about a construct's content equivalence, con-
ceptual property (trait vs. state), theoretical basis (dispositional vs. situ-
ational), and appropriate operationalization (psychometrics vs.
experimental manipulation) (Chen, Gully, Whiteman, & Kilcullen, 2000).
On the other hand, jangle fallacy occurs when different labels are
attached to substantively identical phenomena (Block, 1995). For
instance, the concept labels of future orientation (Das & Teng, 1998)
and (future) temporal depth (Bluedorn, 2002) similarly represent the
time horizon of an individual's consideration into the future. Yet, these
two distinct labels have triggered divergent research trajectories
the former was mostly adopted by strategy and entrepreneurship
research on managerial decisionmaking (e.g., Martin, Wiseman, &
GomezMejia, 2016); the latter was more frequently used by OB
researchers to study employee outcomes (e.g., Bluedorn & Martin,
2008). Such jangle fallacies have hindered knowledge integration
across temporal research.
Second, existing studies are fragmented with respect to levels of
analysis. OB research has placed a skewed emphasis on individual
level studies (Shipp & Cole, 2015). However, scholars contend that
subjective time is an omnipresent phenomenon occurring across mul-
tiple organizational levels, and increasing research has examined tem-
poral constructs at collective levels, especially in adjacent disciplines
(George & Jones, 2000; Mosakowski & Earley, 2000). For example,
polychronicitythe proclivity toward the degree of simultaneity in
performing multiple work tasks (Slocombe & Bluedorn, 1999)has
been studied at the team level both in OB (team polychronicity diver-
sity; Mohammed & Nadkarni, 2014) and in strategy (top management
team (TMT) polychronicity; Souitaris & Maestro, 2010).
Furthermore, many studies examining collective temporal con-
structs did not explain the process of emergence from the individual
level to the higher level. Although both team polychronicity diversity
and TMT polychronicity reflect teamlevel phenomena, they may theo-
retically emerge through different processes (dispersion vs. referent
shift consensus) from individual members' polychronicity and thus
require distinct operationalization approaches (Chan, 1998). Subjec-
tive time involves omnipresent phenomena occurring at multiple orga-
nizational levels, yet scattered temporal research at different levels
and the conceptual imprecision regarding the level and origin of (col-
lective) temporal constructs have severely limited our understanding
of subjective time as a multilevel phenomenon.
To address the fragmented state of research on subjective time,
our review offers an integrative framework that categorizes temporal
constructs based on two generic dimensions: (1) traitversus statelike
property and (2) individual versus collective level of analysis. This, in
combination, yields four archetypes of subjective time: individual tem-
poral disposition, individual temporal state, collective temporal state,
and collective temporal disposition. We clarify the conceptualizations
of temporal constructs belonging to each archetype and review their
key findings in the organizational research.
Our review aims to contribute to the literature in three related
ways. First, we provide greater conceptual clarity, as well as facilitate
the integration of diverse and scattered studies on temporal
constructs. Second, the comparatively broad scope of this review
(incorporating studies at the individual and collective levels, from
OB as well as related disciplines) offers a more encompassing view
of organizational research on subjective time and thereby extends
and complements previously published reviews that have focused
exclusively on individuallevel temporal construct(s) (e.g., Kooij et al.,
2018; Shipp & Cole, 2015). Through the inclusion of both
individualand collectivelevel studies, our ultimate goal is to encour-
age OB researchers to apply a rich, complex, and meatymultilevel
lens (Klein & Kozlowski, 2000) that bridges micro and macro under-
standings of subjective time. Third, we identify critical issues in the
extant literature and provide specific suggestions for future research
to address associated challenges. Overall, we seek to synthesize the
existing research in a systematic and informative manner, which high-
lights major knowledge gaps and charts rich and clear pathways to
move the field forward.
2|LITERATURE REVIEW
2.1 |Scope of the review
We focused our review on subjective time in organizational contexts.
According to McGrath and Rotchford (1983, p. 61), subjective time
has two defining characteristics: (1) It features the subjective nature
of time that is in the eye of the beholderin contrast to objective
time, which is invariant to subjective interpretations; (2) it is consid-
ered a focal construct rather than a medium through which changes
occur
2
.
Because Bluedorn and Denhardt (1988)'s initial review of different
conceptions of time (e.g., objective and subjective) in the field of man-
agement, there has been a burgeoning interest in this topic among
organizational researchers (Shipp & Cole, 2015). Thus, we focused
on scholarly work published after 1988. Our review differs from
recent reviews and metaanalyses (e.g., Kooij et al., 2018; Rudolph,
Kooij, Rauvola, & Zacher, 2018) with respect to our primary goal of
providing conceptual clarification and integration of various temporal
constructs across studies. The broader scope of this review allows us
to complement previously published reviews that predominantly
focused on traitlike, individuallevel phenomena of subjective time
2
As studies on trajectories consider time as objective and the medium through which changes
occur, we did not include them in this review. We refer readers to other reviews on trajecto-
ries including Shipp and Cole (2015) for withinindividual changes and Bush, LePine, and
Newton (2018) for teams in transitions.
TANG ET AL.211

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT