Siblings and Adolescent Weapon Carrying

DOI10.1177/1541204016639354
AuthorLacey N. Wallace
Published date01 July 2017
Date01 July 2017
Subject MatterArticles
Article
Siblings and Adolescent
Weapon Carrying:
Contributions of Genetics,
Shared Environment, and
Nonshared Environment
Lacey N. Wallace
1
Abstract
Many past studies have observed evidence of sibling similarity and influence for delinquency and
substance use. However, studies of sibling similarity for adolescent weapon carrying, particularly for
weapons beyond firearms, are largely absent from the literature. The present study assesses sibling
similarity in weapon carrying as well as the relative contributions of genetics, shared environment,
and nonshared environment. Data are obtained from the first two waves of the National Long-
itudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health and analyzed using biometrical genetic models for twins
and actor–partner interdependence models for nontwins. Results indicate little, if any, contribution
stemming from genetics. There is also no evidence of a significant shared environment effect.
Instead, all or nearly all of the variation and similarity in weapon carrying among siblings are related
to the nonshared environment, particularly gang affiliation. Implications and possible extensions of
these findings are discussed.
Keywords
siblings, weapon carrying, genetics, nonshared environment
Introduction
In 2009, an average of 20 U.S. children and adolescents were hospitalized each day due to firearm
injuries (Leventhal, Gaither, & Sege, 2014). Juvenile offenders account for the majority of violent
crimes against those aged 8–15 (Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention [OJJDP],
2005). As a result, understanding firearm-related behavior among adolescents is one avenue through
1
Department of Criminal Justice, Penn State Altoona, Altoona, PA, USA
Corresponding Author:
Lacey N. Wallace, Department of Criminal Justice, Penn State Altoona, 101G Cypress Building, 3000 Ivyside Park, Altoona,
PA 16601, USA.
Email: lno106@psu.edu
Youth Violence and JuvenileJustice
2017, Vol. 15(3) 264-280
ªThe Author(s) 2016
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1541204016639354
journals.sagepub.com/home/yvj
which these crimes can potentially be prevented. The present article focuses on adolescent weapon
carrying, which can include both firearms and other weapon types. Nearly 18%of respondents (28%
of males) to the 2013 High School Youth Risk Behavior Survey reported carrying a weapon at some
point in the past month; more than 5%reported carrying a gun (Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention, 2014). Although less common, some adolescents also bring weapons to school. More
than 5%of respondents to the 2013 High School Youth Risk Behavior Survey reported carrying a
gun, knife, or club to school in the past 30 days (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2014).
Approximately 7%of respondents reported that they were threatened or injured with a weapon on
school property at least once in the past year (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2014).
Unfortunately, much is still unknown about peer and sibling influence for this problematic behavior.
This study focuses on the role of siblings in particular. Sibling concordance of delinquent
behavior has been well established in the literature (Slomkowski, Rende, Conger, Simons, & Con-
ger, 2001; Tuvblad & Baker, 2011; Whiteman, Jensen, & Maggs, 2013). Lauritsen’s (1993)
research, for example, indicated that adolescent delinquency was concentrated among households;
10%of households in the sample accounted for 76%of all delinquent acts reported (Lauritsen, 1993,
p. 399). Similarly, Rowe, Rodgers, and Meseck-Bushey (1992) found that the correlation for delin-
quency in same-sex sibling dyads was roughly .30 for brothers, .28 for sisters, and .21 for mixed-sex
sibling pairs. Although these trends hold for various forms of delinquency, sibling similarity for
weapon carrying has not been underexplored.
Past research, however, indicates two key explanations for why similarity in this behavior might
be expected. The first, based on the principles of social learning, simply suggests that siblings
influence one another through imitation and modeling as well as through the process of interaction
(Akers, 2009; Whiteman et al., 2013). In other words, if one sibling carries a weapon, another sibling
may perceive this as acceptable or desirable and imitate the behavior. Since siblings report similar, if
not identical, access to guns in the home (Mocan & Tekin, 2006), this possibility is certainly
plausible. A second explanation is heritability. Research on aggression has found that roughly half
of the variation in aggressive behavior can be attributed to genetics (Tuvblad & Baker, 2011). Both
explanations suggest that siblings should be similar in weapon carrying behavior.
In contrast, some authors argue that siblings may be greatly influenced by nonshared environ-
ments. More than 40%of the respondents assessed by Daniels and Plomin (1985), for instance,
reported experiences that differed from those of their siblings in areas including parental treatment,
peer groups, and subjective exp eriences of events such as divo rce, romantic relationships, a nd
family problems. These differences were not significantly related to genetics and were believed
to originate with environmental variation either inside or outside of the home (Daniels & Plomin,
1985). If these nonshared experiences are indeed the driving factors behind adolescent weapon
carrying, then divergence between siblings would be expected rather than similarity. Unfortunately,
little research has empirically evaluated these possibilities.
One study that has addressed sibling similarity in weapon carrying found that 27%of variation in
weapon carrying is attributable to genetics (Connolly & Beaver, 2015). However, the study only
considered handgun carrying. Estimates for weapon carrying beyond handguns are unknown. To
address this omission, the present study examines three key questions. First, do siblings have similar
school weapon carrying frequencies? Second, to what extent does the weapon carrying of one sibling
in adolescence influence the subsequent weapon carrying behavior of another sibling? Third, how
does weapon carrying behavior among sibling pairs differ by genetic relatedness (R)? Together,
these questions address the degree of sibling similarity for weapon carrying as well as the origins of
similarity among siblings for this behavior.
The answers to these questions are important for youth violence prevention. Existing prevention
efforts focus on factors that influence juveniles outside of the home, such as gangs and peers. Little
research has investigated the possibility that weapon carrying might originate from influences within
Wallace 265

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT