Sherlock Holmes and the Case of Resist/Refuse Dynamics: Confirmatory Bias and Abductive Inference in Child Custody Evaluations

DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/fcre.12478
Published date01 April 2020
Date01 April 2020
AuthorBenjamin D. Garber
SPECIAL ISSUE: PARENT-CHILD CONTACT PROBLEMS: CONCEPTS,
CONTROVERSIES, & CONUNDRUMS
CASE EVALUATION AND RESPONSE
SHERLOCK HOLMES AND THE CASE OF RESIST/REFUSE
DYNAMICS: CONFIRMATORY BIAS AND ABDUCTIVE INFERENCE IN
CHILD CUSTODY EVALUATIONS
Benjamin D. Garber
Had Sir Arthur Conan Doylesctional detective, the great Sherlock Holmes, actually engaged in deductive reasoning, hewould
have solved many fewer crimes.In fact, Holmeslogical progression from astute observation to hypotheses is a model of a type
of inductive reasoning. This paper argues that mental health professionals tasked to evaluate why a child is resisting/refusing con-
tact with one parent must approach each family the way that Holmes approached each case, without a presumed suspect, moving
systematically from detail to hypothesis, well-versed inthe full range of dynamicsthat may be at play, and erring in favor of par-
simony rather than pathology. By contrast, the custody evaluator who approaches these matters through a deductive process, seek-
ing data that support an aprioritheory, is vulnerable to conrmatory bias and doing harm to the child whose interests are
paramount. The literature concerned with resist/refuse dyna mics is reviewed, yielding 13 non-mutually exclusive variables that
evaluators must consider so as to more fully identify why a particular child is resisting or refusing contact with one parent. On
this basis, the hybrid model is expanded to include th e full spectrum of contributing dynamics. Specic recommendations are
made for judicial ofcers in the interest of writing orders for custody evaluations that minimize the risk of conrmatory bias.
Key Points for the Family Court Community:
Deductive reasoning seeks to conrm or refute an a priori hypothesis
Deductive reasoning is highly vulnerable to conrmational bias
Conrmational bias can corrupt and invalidate forensic evaluation to the detriment of all involved
Resist/refuse dynamics must be understood through an inductive process that is open to all possible hypotheses
A survey of the literature identies at least thirteen mutually compatible hypotheses, all of which must be evaluated
Courts must take care to wordorders for forensic family evaluations in a manner that minimizes conrmatory bias and
invites inductiveinvestigation
Keywords: Alienation; Child Custody; Conrmatory Bias; Co-parenting; Divorce; Enmeshment; Estrangement; Polariza-
tion; Resist/Refuse; Triangulation.
I suppose it is tempting,
if the only tool you have is a hammer,
to treat everything as if it were a nail.
Maslow (1966) (p. 15).
By denition, child custody evaluators are asked to engage in a sort of retrospective detective
work. Given a familys dysfunction and distress in the present, one must infer past causes so as to
either intervene as a Court Involved Therapist (Fidnick, Koch, Greenberg, & Sullivan, 2011) or
Corresponding: bdgarberphd@familylawconsulting.org
The author is honored to have been invited to prepare this article for a special edition of Family Court Review to appear in
04.2020
FAMILY COURT REVIEW, Vol. 58 No. 2, April 2020 386402
© 2020 Association of Family and Conciliation Courts
advise the court in the capacity of Guardian ad litem (GAL), court-appointed child custody evalu-
ator, or party retained testamentary expert (Gould, Dale, Fisher, & Gould, 2016). Logic dictates that
this task can be approached in one of two ways. The deductive process presumes a hypothesis about
causation and proceeds to gather data that will either conrm or refute it. The inductive process
begins by gathering data and, on the basis of those agglomerated observations, only then infers
competing hypotheses that might thereafter be tested.
This paper describes the deductive process as incompatible with the court-appointed child cus-
tody evaluators ethics, leaving him or her vulnerable to conrmatory bias and thereby doing harm
to children. By contrast, the inductive process is congruent with the child custody evaluators ethical
guidelines and more likely to generate an opinion that is impartial and genuinely child centered.
The literature concerned with resist/refuse dynamics is reviewed, yielding 13 non-mutually exclu-
sive variables relevant to assessing resist/refuse dynamics. The child custody evaluator is advised
that each of the 13 variables must be weighed so as to induce the unique formula or recipe that
describes the dynamics associated with a particular childs polarized position in the conicted
family.
1
I. DEDUCTION, INDUCTION, AND ABDUCTION
Most people associate deduction with Sherlock Holmes, Sir Arthur Conan Doyles brilliant and
daring ctional detective. In a typical mystery, Holmes visits the scene of a crime. He observes
details others failed to note, and applying these data to his encyclopedic grasp of the possible
dynamics in play, he generates a who-done-it hypothesis. For all of the characters fame and skill,
this process of reasoning backwards(Conan Doyle, 1888) is not, in fact, deductive. Deduction is
a top-down process of inference that seeks to conrm or refute a preliminar y hypothesis. Deduction
is the second step of the scientic method, the means with which hypotheses induced from observa-
tion are refuted or conrmed. Holmes pointedly took on each investigation without presumption.
He rigorously (and sometimes tediously) studied the facts and only then offered a hypothesis. Build-
ing his case from the bottom-up, his method is the prototype of inductive reasoning.
To illustrate: were Holmes the master of deduc tion that he is commonly thought to be, he would
have approached each murder scene with a hypothesis already in mind, for example, in the persev-
erative belief that his arch enemy, Moriarty, had struck again. He would then have gathered evi-
dence relevant to that hypothesis. In so doing, he likely would have been blind to the minutiae that
a more open-minded, inductive process relies upon (e.g., a dogs failure to bark [Conan Doyle,
1892]). He would thereby have been a far less successful detective (Carson, 2009).
Inductive logic describes the process that physicians use in the course of generating preliminary
diagnoses. The physician collects symptoms the same way that Holmes collects clues. When a pat-
tern is recognized, hypotheses are generated in the form of potential diagnoses. Further testing
(i.e., deduction) is then conducted to rule in or rule out these competing hypotheses. The initial
inductive process in medicine is distinct, however, from the inductive process that Holmes engaged
in to the extent that the physicians set of possible hypotheses is nite and familiar. Nosologies such
as the International Classication of Diseases (World Health Organization, 1993) effectively create
a menu of options.
Inductive reasoning in a context in which the eld of possible hypotheses is nite and familiar
is known as abduction (not to be confused with kidnapping [Rapezzi, Ferrari, & Branzi, 2005]).
2
Where Sherlock Holmesreasoning is best identied as inductive in that the pool of poss ible per-
petrators was effectively limitl ess and unknowable, murder myst eries in which the pool of p ossible
perpetrators i s well established are abduc tive. Agatha Christie, for ex ample, asks readers to eng age
in an exercise in abduct ion in her renowned play, And Then There Were None(Christie,
1939).
3
Garber/CONFIRMATORY BIAS AND RESIST/REFUSE DYNAMICS IN CCES 387

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT