Sentencing on the Evidence

AuthorElizabeth Griffiths,Esther Nir
Published date01 May 2018
Date01 May 2018
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1177/0887403416635248
Subject MatterArticles
Criminal Justice Policy Review
2018, Vol. 29(4) 365 –390
© The Author(s) 2016
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0887403416635248
journals.sagepub.com/home/cjp
Article
Sentencing on the Evidence
Esther Nir1 and Elizabeth Griffiths1
Abstract
The type and quantity of evidence in a case is a critical factor for deciding guilt but
should have little or no influence on the sentencing determinations of judges post
conviction; this is because case evidence goes to guilt decisions by triers of fact,
whereas sentences are imposed upon those already convicted. This study examines
the effects of evidentiary type and the total quantity of physical evidence in a case on
length of custodial sentence. The results demonstrate that violent felony cases with
forensic evidence and those cases with more varied pieces of physical evidence result
in longer custodial sentences for convicted defendants. Thus, the findings indicate that
inculpatory evidence in criminal trials has enduring effects post conviction and, more
broadly, suggest that judicial discretion at sentencing is, at least in part, influenced by
the judge’s confidence in the defendant’s guilt.
Keywords
sentence length, evidence, sentencing disparity, forensic, punishment
Criminologists and legal scholars have routinely lamented the substantial sentencing
disparities that exist in the processing of criminal cases. Despite having been con-
victed of like offenses, defendants with similar criminal backgrounds tried in federal
and state court systems receive widely disparate sentences (Ulmer, 2012; Wooldredge,
2010). Various rationales have been posited for these disparities, including the struc-
ture of sentencing guidelines (Frankel, 1972; Kramer & Ulmer, 1996), the mode of
conviction (N. J. King, Soulé, Steen, & Weidner, 2005; Piehl & Bushway, 2007), racial
and ethnic discrimination (Albonetti, 1997; Scott, 2011; Steffensmeier & Demuth,
2000), local organizational constraints (Ulmer, 2012), and various idiosyncrasies in
the discretionary power of judges, among other sources of difference (Frankel, 1972;
Wooldredge, 2010). With respect to judicial discretion, history shows that, of the many
1Rutgers University–Newark, NJ, USA
Corresponding Author:
Esther Nir, School of Criminal Justice, Rutgers University–Newark, 123 Washington Street, Newark, NJ
07102, USA.
Email: esther.nir@rutgers.edu
635248CJPXXX10.1177/0887403416635248Criminal Justice Policy ReviewNir and Grifths
research-article2016
366 Criminal Justice Policy Review 29(4)
relevant sentencing factors, “each is capable of being answered, and is answered by
sentencing judges, in contradictory or conflicting or at least differing ways” (Frankel,
1972, p. 5). Indeed,
judges carry their own experiences, biases, and foibles with them into the role; they vary
widely in their explicit views and “principles” affecting sentencing; they vary, too, in the
accidents of birth and biography generating the guilts, the fears, and the rages that affect
almost all of us at times and in ways we often cannot know. (p. 7)1
But whatever their causes, sentencing disparities result in a host of negative conse-
quences, including the uneven administration of punishment, perceptions that the sys-
tem is unjust, and resentment among prisoners, among others (Lynch, 2009; Tonry,
1996; Walker, 1993).
In theory, sentencing determinations should rely on legally relevant sentencing fac-
tors, including the seriousness of the offense and the defendant’s prior criminal record
(Roberts, 1994; von Hirsch, 1992). In practice, however, extralegal factors such as the
defendant’s race, age, and gender have been found to influence the punitiveness of
sentences (Doerner & Demuth, 2010; Feldmeyer & Ulmer, 2011; Johnson, Stewart,
Pickett, & Gertz, 2011; R. D. Peterson & Hagan, 1984). Although less attention has
been directed at it as a source of disparity, there is some evidence that the confidence
a judge has in a jury’s verdict may also affect the length of imposed sentences (Gertner,
1999). For example, in the case of U.S. v. Juarez-Ortega (1989), codefendants Juarez-
Ortega and DeLuna were charged and tried together for two counts of distributing
cocaine and one count of carrying a firearm during a drug trafficking offense; the
weapons charge mandated a 5-year minimum consecutive sentence. DeLuna was con-
victed of all three charges, whereas Juarez-Ortega was convicted on both cocaine
charges but acquitted on the weapons charge. Yet the judge imposed identical sen-
tences upon each defendant. In justifying the sentence, the judge stated,
I have been disappointed in jury verdicts before but that’s one of the most important ones,
because what it did, it set up a disparity in result between the two defendants . . . This
firearm was used. [The jury] had to absolutely disregard the testimony of the government
agent for no reason—no reason. (U.S. v. Juarez-Ortega, 1989, pp. 748-749)
Thus, the judge’s disagreement with the jury’s verdict for Juarez-Ortega resulted in
both defendants being subject to the same penalty despite being convicted on varied
counts (Gertner, 1999). The outcome of U.S. v. Juarez-Ortega suggests that judicial
confidence in the correctness of a jury’s guilty verdict may influence sentence
severity.2
Confidence in the verdict may be partially or even primarily a function of the
judge’s evaluation of the evidence garnered against the defendant. Indeed, a small
body of research has spread beyond the characteristics of defendants and victims,
courtroom working groups, and social and organizational contexts to explore the role
of case-specific features, including strength of evidence, on case disposition, and, to a

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT