ROLE OF THE STREET NETWORK IN BURGLARS' SPATIAL DECISION‐MAKING*

Date01 May 2017
Published date01 May 2017
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/1745-9125.12133
ROLE OF THE STREET NETWORK IN BURGLARS’
SPATIAL DECISION-MAKING
MICHAEL J. FRITH,1SHANE D. JOHNSON,1
and HANNAH M. FRY2
1Department of Security and Crime Science, University College London
2Bartlett Centre for Advanced Spatial Analysis, University College London
KEYWORDS: burglary, road network, graph theory, discrete choice, mixed logit
Explaining why crime is spatially concentrated has been a central theme of much
criminological research. Although various theories focus on neighborhood social pro-
cesses, environmental criminology asserts that the physical environment plays a central
role by shaping people’s activity patterns and the opportunities for crime. Here, we test
theoretical expectations regarding the role of the road network in shaping the spatial
distribution of crime and, in contrast to prior research, disentangle how it might influ-
ence offender awareness of criminal opportunities and the supply of ambient guardian-
ship. With a mixed logit (discrete choice) model, we use data regarding (N=459)
residential burglaries (for the first time) to model offender spatial decision-making at
the street segment level. Novel graph theory metrics are developed to estimate offender
awareness of street segments and to estimate levels of ambient guardianship, distin-
guishing between local and nonlocal guardianship. As predicted by crime pattern the-
ory, novel metrics concerning offender familiarity and effort were significant predictors
of residential burglary location choices. And, in line with Newman’s (1972) concept of
defensible space, nonlocal (local) pedestrian traffic was found to be associated with an
increase (decrease) in burglary risk. Our findings also demonstrate that “taste” prefer-
ences vary across offenders, which presents a challenge for future research to explain.
That crime is spatially concentrated now seems incontestable (e.g., Eck and Weisburd,
1995). Explaining why this is so, however, is still a central theme of criminological research
and a matter of some debate. Several prevailing theories assert that the environment
plays a central role in shaping the distribution of crime by facilitating the convergence
in space and time of offenders and suitable targets, in the absence of capable guardians
(Cohen and Felson, 1979). One fundamental determinant of this is the road network
because it defines how people move through the urban environment. In so doing, it serves
Additional supporting information can be found in the listing for this article in the Wiley Online
Library at http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/crim.2017.55.issue-2/issuetoc.
This project was funded by EPSRC grant no. EP/G037264/1 as part of the UCL Department of
Security and Crime Science’s Doctoral Training Centre.
Direct correspondence to Shane D. Johnson, Department of Security and Crime Sci-
ence, University College London, 35 Tavistock Square, London WC1H 9EZ, U.K. (e-mail:
shane.johnson@ucl.ac.uk).
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which per-
mits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
C2017 The Authors. Criminology published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. on behalf of American Society of
Criminology doi: 10.1111/1745-9125.12133
CRIMINOLOGY Volume 55 Number 2 344–376 2017 344
OFFENDER SPATIAL DECISION-MAKING 345
a “dual function,” determining the opportunities for crime offenders encounter and be-
come aware of (e.g., Beavon, Brantingham, and Brantingham, 1994), and influencing the
locations through which ordinary citizens move and provide ambient guardianship. The
aim of prior research has been to examine the role of the road network on crime, but it
has failed to isolate the influence of these two mechanisms. This is largely because analy-
ses have been conducted to examine only the location of crime events, without reference
to the offenders involved. In addition, most researchers have tended to employ crude
metrics to describe the network, and their findings have often lacked statistical rigor.
To address these shortcomings, in this article, we make several novel contributions.
We use a discrete choice approach (McFadden, 1974), or more accurately an offense lo-
cation choice approach (Bernasco and Nieuwbeerta, 2005), to estimate empirically how
the opportunities that are targeted by burglars differ from those that are not. Our re-
search differs from previous studies of offender spatial decision-making in two important
ways. First, in most previous studies, scholars have examined offender location choice
at the area level (for the exceptions, see Bernasco, 2010b, and Vandeviver et al., 2015).
Here, consistent with contemporary theory (Weisburd, Groff, and Yang, 2012) and the
research questions at hand, we do so at the street segment level. Second, we build on
previous work (Davies and Johnson, 2015) and introduce to the discrete choice literature
a methodological approach that aims to estimate independently how the road network
influences offender awareness of crime opportunities, on the one hand, and guardian po-
tential at particular locations, on the other. By following Townsley, Birks, Ruiter, et al.
(2015), we also recognize that offenders may vary in the extent to which their spatial
decision-making is affected by different factors, and so we employ mixed logit statistical
models to estimate parameters and how they vary across offenders. In combination, our
approach allows us to examine the dual role that the road network might play in shaping
burglar spatial decision-making and the extent to which this varies across offenders.
The remainder of this article is organized as follows. In the next section, theoretical per-
spectives and research are reviewed to introduce the theoretical model. The second and
third sections describe the data and analytic strategy, respectively. The latter includes a
discussion of the application of graph theory to quantify the character of the road net-
work, as well as the statistical model employed. The fourth and fifth sections present and
discuss the results.
BACKGROUND
OFFENDERS AND THE ROAD NETWORK
The rational choice perspective (Cornish and Clarke, 1986) describes offenders, such
as burglars, as nonarbitrary decision-makers who consider (however briefly) the costs
and benefits of action alternatives, including the decision of where to offend (Clarke and
Felson, 1993). It is suggested that such decision-making will be bounded by imperfect and
incomplete information, and that future choices will be informed by the outcome of previ-
ous ones (Cornish and Clarke, 1986; see also Bennett and Wright, 1984; Cromwell, Olson,
and Avary, 1991; Wright and Decker, 1994). Nevertheless, although choices made may
not appear rational to an observer, it is assumed that selections are made that aim to max-
imize the perceived utility of a decision and minimize the expected costs. In particular, the
distance an offender must travel to offend has consistently been shown by researchers to
influence offender location choice in both quantitative and ethnographic studies.
346 FRITH, JOHNSON, & FRY
For example, offenders have consistently reported (e.g., Brown and Altman, 1981;
Rengert and Wasilchick, 1985; Repetto, 1974), and the results of analyses of the jour-
ney to crime (Snook, 2004; Wiles and Costello, 2000) have shown, that the likelihood of
an offender selecting a location at which to offend is inversely proportional to the dis-
tance he or she must travel to reach it. The aim of most studies of the journey to crime
has been to examine the Euclidean distance between locations (e.g., Clare, Fernandez,
and Morgan, 2009), and even though this provides a good estimate of the likely cost of
travel, it is clearly imperfect. The cost of travel is intrinsically linked to the configuration
and properties (such as the vehicle speed limit) of the road network as this determines
how quick and how easy it is to travel between any two locations. Consequently, in the
current study, we expect to find the following:
Hypothesis 1: Street segments that are quicker to travel to (in terms of estimated
travel time) from the burglar’s home will be more likely to be selected for burglary.
In addition to the fact that the selection of targets near to an offender’s home location
minimizes the cost of travel, this preference is expected to emerge for other reasons en-
capsulated by crime pattern theory (e.g., Brantingham and Brantingham, 1993). Accord-
ing to the theory, like everyone else, offenders are assumed to frequent activity nodes
routinely, such as their home and workplace. As a consequence of doing so, they develop
an awareness of these places and the routes between them. This is not necessary for of-
fending, but it shapes offenders’ familiarity and awareness of the criminal opportunities
within these spaces. According to crime pattern theory, offenders are believed to prefer
such opportunities to alternative targets for two reasons. First, offenders cannot select tar-
gets of which they are not aware (Rengert and Wasilchick, 1985). And, second, targeting
locations about which something is known reduces uncertainty about the likely outcome
of that choice (Beavon, Brantingham, and Brantingham, 1994). As such, we predict the
following:
Hypothesis 2: Street segments that are more likely to be familiar to an offender are
more likely to be selected for burglary.
In previous studies, scholars have assumed that the distance between a location and an
offender’s routine activity nodes (e.g., the home) provides a reasonable estimate of that
location’s familiarity. This assumption is not unreasonable as the findings from qualita-
tive research with burglars have revealed that they are typically most familiar with areas
that are closest to their home locations (e.g., Rengert and Wasilchick, 1985). Neverthe-
less, at the street segment level, an individual’s familiarity with locations will be more
nuanced than this, and it is likely to be a function of how frequently he or she travels to
or through them (Brantingham and Brantingham, 1993; Rengert and Wasilchick, 1985).
Although distance will influence this awareness, so too will the configuration of the road
network because this affects the likelihood that an individual will travel along a particu-
lar street to reach that or other locations (e.g., Beavon, Brantingham, and Brantingham,
1994). Some streets (such as major roads) may feature in many journeys, whereas oth-
ers less so (cul de sacs and dead ends, for instance). On the whole, streets rarely trav-
eled will be less familiar than those commonly used. Even though it is not possible to
determine which streets offenders travel during their routine activities without collect-
ing extensive primary data, it is possible to employ techniques from graph theory to

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT