Retracted: The Development of Willingness to Sacrifice and Unmitigated Communion in Intimate Partnerships

DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12455
AuthorRebecca M. Horne,Franz J. Neyer,Matthew D. Johnson
Published date01 June 2018
Date01 June 2018
RETRACTED
M D. J University of Alberta
R M. H University of Alberta
F J. N Friedrich-Schiller-Universität Jena∗∗
The Development of Willingness to Sacrice and
Unmitigated Communion in Intimate Partnerships
Rooted in a relational developmental systems
perspective and drawing on German Family
Panel data from 3,405 focal participants and
their partners surveyed for 7 years, this study
examined developmental trajectories of willing-
ness to sacrice and unmitigated communion,
or putting a partner’s needs above one’s own
to the exclusion of the self. Findings revealed
each construct followed a declining curvilin-
ear pattern with much variability underlying
average trajectories. Women, those with more
prior partnerships, and those in longer term
relationships were less willing to sacrice and
had lower unmitigated communion at baseline.
Participants were more willing to sacrice
when they were more committed, when they and
their partners were happier with the union, and
when household income was lower.Unmitigated
Department of Human Ecology, Universityof Alberta, 302
Human Ecology Building, Edmonton, AB T6G 2N1,
Canada (matt.johnson@ualberta.ca).
Department of Human Ecology, Universityof Alberta, 302
Human Ecology Building, Edmonton, AB T6G 2N1,
Canada. Current address: Department of Psychology,
University of Toronto,100 St. George Street, Sidney Smith
Hall 4th Floor, Toronto,ON M58 3G3, Canada.
∗∗Department of Psychology, Friedrich-Schiller-
Universität Jena, Humboldtstraße 11, Raum 118, 07743
Jena, Germany.
Key Words: commitment, couple relations, life span theory,
longitudinal, relationship cognitions, relationship develop-
ment.
communion was higher when participants were
more committed and more insecure in their
partner’s love. Willingness to sacrice and
unmitigated communion develop in response to
sociodemographics, personal history, and both
partners’ relationship cognitions.
The willingness to sacrice immediate desires
to benet one’s intimate partner or relationship
has proven a critical component for the attain-
ment of lasting love. Much sophisticated work
has examined the impact of sacrice on couple
relations, nding greater willingness to and
satisfaction with sacrice exhibited robust links
with more satisfying and stable couple unions
(e.g., Stanley, Whitton, Sadberry, Clements,
& Markman, 2006; Van Lange et al., 1997),
especially when the sacrice is prompted by a
genuine desire to please one’spartner or improve
the relationship (Impett, Gable, & Peplau, 2005;
Impett, Gere, Kogan, Gordon, & Keltner, 2013).
Sacrice has a particularly potent impact on
couple relations because it provides a clear
signal of one’s commitment (Stanley, Rhoades,
& Whitton, 2010; Van Lange et al., 1997).
Indeed, when partners believe the other is will-
ing to sacrice, trust and commitment increases,
prompting a greater willingness to sacrice
and increased pro-relationship acts, setting a
mutual cyclical growth process into motion
(Wieselquist, Rusbult, Foster, & Agnew, 1999).
Forgoing one’s own self-interest for a partner
is certainly an important contributor to satisfying
Journal of Marriage and Family 80 (June 2018): 637–652 637
DOI:10.1111/jomf.12455
RETRACTED
638 Journal of Marriage and Family
couple relations, but sacrice does not always
benet relationships. Sacrices made to avoid
a negative event, such as conict with a partner
(Impett et al., 2005, 2013), and believing sacri-
ces are personally harmful (Whitton, Stanley,
& Markman, 2007) may erode relationship
quality. Along these lines, oversacricing for
one’s partner may also represent one aspect
of sacrice that is not a boon to relational
health. Personality psychologists have long
examined people’s tendency to oversacrice
(outside the context of couple unions), referred
to as unmitigated communion, or a tendency
to excessively put the needs of others above
one’s own to the exclusion of the self (Helgeson
& Fritz, 1998). Unmitigated communion was
associated with reduced individual well-being
in prior research, including low self-esteem and
heightened depression (Fritz & Helgeson, 1998)
and poorer physical health (Helgeson, Swanson,
Ra, Randall, & Zhao, 2015). Despite compelling
arguments articulating its salience for couple
unions (Impett & Gordon, 2008), unmitigated
communion is yet to be examined in the context
of couple relations, representing an important
and novel contribution of this study.
Given the importance of willingness to sac-
rice and unmitigated communion for personal
and relationship well-being, understanding their
developmental trajectories and the factors that
motivate growth in each is essential to further
articulate how couples might achieve lasting
love. Yet we could locate no research with this
aim. Drawing on developmental theory and
four waves of survey data provided by 3,405
focal participants, referred to as anchors, and
their partners in the German Family Panel (pair-
fam; Brüderl et al., 2016), this study examines
trajectories of intimate partnership-oriented
willingness to sacrice (“I am usually willing
to sacrice my own desires for my partner”)
and unmitigated communion (“Often, I leave
everything else aside to support my partner”)
and whether gender moderates these trajectories
and explores covariates that might shape the
development of these constructs.
B
Theoretical Framework
A relational developmental systems (RDS;
Lerner, Agans, DeSouza, & Gasca, 2013;
Lerner, Johnson, & Buckingham, 2015)per-
spective guides our examination of willingness
to sacrice and unmitigated communion tra-
jectories and the factors that may impact these
developmental processes. At its core, a RDS
perspective stresses that development is driven
by the recurrent, mutual inuence between indi-
viduals and the many environments in which
they exist. To achieve the RDS stated goal
of unearthing knowledge that can lead to the
optimization of human development (Lerner
et al., 2013), research must rst describe and
explain various aspects of human development.
The description of human development involves
examining change processes reected in indi-
vidual trajectories observed across time, which
may feature elements of growth, decline, or
stability and exploring whether developmental
processes unfold in concert with one another or
assume generally distinct patterns.
Applied to the present study, a RDS perspec-
tive motivates the need to understand how will-
ingness to sacrice and unmitigated communion
develop while considering whether the trajecto-
ries of these relationship cognitions are linked.
We could locate no research examining the
long-term development of these variables. The
longitudinal work in this area has examined the
interrelation of sacrice with relational and indi-
vidual constructs using daily diary (Helgeson
et al., 2015; Impett et al., 2005; Ruppel & Cur-
ran, 2012) and panel data (M. D. Johnson &
Horne, 2016) or has examined consequences of
unmitigated communion and sacrice on future
personal and relational well-being (Aubé, 2008;
Impett et al., 2005, 2013; Stanley, Whitton,
et al., 2006; Van Lange et al., 1997). The data
contain four biennial assessments of willingness
to sacrice and unmitigated communion from a
large, diverse sample of partnered individuals,
making them ideally suited for examining the
longer term development of each construct.
Development does not transpire in isolation,
and explaining human development necessitates
consideration of intrapersonal and contextual
factors that motivate change. One proximal and
enduring contextual factor is an intimate partner,
as couple ties are characterized by high levels of
contact, emotional connectedness, and interde-
pendence (Huston, 2000). In the present work,
an RDS perspective not only leads us to consider
whether anchors’ past experiences (number of
prior intimate partnerships), sociodemographic
characteristics (e.g., gender, relationship length,
household income, number of children, and
relationship status), and relationship-oriented

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT