Receptivity to Research in Policing

AuthorCynthia Lum,Cody W. Telep,Julie Grieco,Christopher S. Koper
Published date01 June 2012
DOI10.3818/JRP.14.1.2012.61
Date01 June 2012
Subject MatterSpecial Issue on Evidence-Based Policy and Practice

*

Receptivitytoresearchinpolicing
Cynthia Lum
Cody W. Telep
Christopher S. Koper
Julie Grieco
Center for Evidence-Based Crime Policy
Department of Criminology, Law and Society
George Mason University
* Abstract
Evidence-based policing—using research and scientif‌ic processes to inform police de-
cisions—is a complex approach to policing that involves various challenges. One pri-
mary diff‌iculty is how research can be translated into digestible and familiar forms for
practitioners. A central part of successful translation is the receptivity of decisionmak-
ers to research as well as how research is presented and packaged to increase receptiv-
ity. In this article we f‌irst discuss the complexity of evidence-based policing, highlight-
ing the much-lamented gap between research and practice. We review research from
other disciplines and also in policing about what contributes to research being better
received and used by practitioners. We then describe our own receptivity survey, offer-
ing preliminary f‌indings about the receptivity of off‌icers to research, researchers, and
tactics inf‌luenced by research. Finally, we conclude with examples of the types of efforts
practitioners and researchers can engage in that might improve receptivity to research.
Specif‌ically, we discuss the Evidence-Based Policing Matrix as a research translation
tool, as well as multiple demonstrations conducted by the authors that focus on insti-
tutionalizing the use of research into daily police activities.
The authors would like to thank the Sacramento Police Department, and especially Sgt.
Renee Mitchell, for their efforts in administering the research receptivity survey. Thanks also
to Julie Hibdon, the JRP editors and anonymous reviewers for their comments, and Jaspreet
Chahal and Julie Wan for their research and editorial assistance.
JUSTICE RESEARCH AND POLICY, Vol. 14, No. 1, 2012
© 2012 Justice Research and Statistics Association
Sp e c i a l iS S u e o n ev i d e n c e -Ba S e d po l i c y a n d pr a c t i c e
P

In his 1998 Ideas in American Policing lecture for the Police Foundation, Lawrence
Sherman stated that “police practices should be based on scientif‌ic evidence about
what works best” (Sherman, 1998, p. 2). Sherman described two dimensions of a
research orientation in policing: the use of information from evaluations of police
activities and the application of knowledge arising from an agency’s own internal
analysis. He emphasized that the police should use scientif‌ically rigorous evalua-
tions and research in a more direct and central way, arguing that research f‌indings
and data analysis should guide police decisions about tactics and strategies.
While this approach seems rational and straightforward, Sherman was not argu-
ing that the road to evidence-based policing is an easy one to follow. Evidence-based
policing, like many policing perspectives, involves complexity and nuance. Those
who support this approach are far from asserting that researchers, research, or sci-
entif‌ic processes can run a police department’s daily operations or resolve law en-
forcement’s concerns, as some have implied (e.g., Sparrow, 2011). Just as the SARA
model (scanning, analysis, response, and assessment) of problem-oriented policing
(Eck & Spelman, 1987) cannot be expected to be used for all of the activities in
which the police engage, and just as community policing is hampered by political
and resource constraints, evidence-based policing also has limitations. Why? Be-
cause evidence-based policing is a decisionmaking perspective, not a panacea. It is
grounded in the idea that policies and practices should be supported by scientif‌ically
rigorous evidence and analytics; that research is not ignored; and that research at
least becomes a part of the conversation about what to do about reducing crime,
increasing legitimacy, and addressing internal problems. These nuances provide f‌lex-
ibility in thinking about the role that research and science should play in policing.
Making research a part of the conversation on policing is complicated by the fact
that two entities (the scientist and the practitioner) with different expectations and
worldviews are attempting to foster and sustain exchanges with one another in order
to trade knowledge, skills, and products. These differences can result in divergent
interpretations of that knowledge and, more generally, different philosophies about
the role and meaning of science in policing. Scientists and practitioners may also
disagree on which outputs best measure police effectiveness (e.g., crime reduction
or crime detection), how evaluations should be carried out (e.g., experiments, quasi-
experiments, simulations, or before/after designs), or what “good policing” should
look like (Mastrofski, Willis, & Revier, 2011). The worlds of the practitioner and the
scientist operate on vastly different timelines, with police chiefs believing that they
need quick solutions, and academics believing that without adequate deliberation,
the quality of the science might be compromised. These many diff‌iculties can some-
times result in either the researcher or the practitioner conceding defeat or simply
avoiding the relationship, which then manifests itself as the proverbial gap between
research and practice (Lum, 2009; Sherman, 1998, 2011; Weisburd, 2008).
At the same time, some police and research personnel are committed to fos-
tering such conversation and see the value of public policy and social interven-
tions being informed by science rather than by hunches, best guesses, or even

“best
prac
tices” (Lum, 2009). This mutual belief is ref‌lected in a history of police-
research partnerships, as well as initiatives at the federal level to fund such partner-
ships (see the report on this topic by the International Association of Chiefs of Po-
lice [IACP], 2004). Recent examples of federal support for these partnerships are
the Bureau of Justice Assistance’s Smart Policing Initiative (Medaris & Huntoon,
2009) and the National Institute of Justice’s Building and Enhancing Criminal
Justice Researcher-Practitioner Partnerships solicitation.1
This interest in reducing the barriers between research and practice is certainly
not a new pursuit in modern democratic societies. When Carol Weiss (with Mi-
chael Bucuvalas) wrote Social Science Research and Decision-Making in 1980, she
pointed out that numerous commissions and inquiries by the National Research
Council (NRC) and the National Science Foundation had already been undertaken
to examine the limited impact of research in the social sciences. And, she wrote, even
the most optimistic felt that the “potential of social science research for informing
the processes of government … has not been realized” (p. 9; see also Hirschkorn
& Geelan, 2008). In the evaluation discipline since, there has been much debate
and discussion over the utilization of research (for a review, see Shulha & Cousins,
1997). Twenty years after her study, Weiss (1998) addressed the American Evalua-
tion Association and again offered cautious optimism. In response to the question
posed by the title of her speech, Have We Learned Anything New About the Use of
Evaluation? she answers, “yes, we have learned some things, but the learnings have
come more from applying new constructs and perspectives than from research on
evaluation use” (p. 23). Nutley, Walter and Davies (2007) in their excellent work
Using Evidence: How Research Can Inform Public Services also emphasize the lack
of empirical evidence on the various models and conceptualizations of research use.
In policing, concern over the gap between research and practice also seems to
be a recurring lament. Bayley (1998) bluntly stated that “research may not have
made as signif‌icant, or at least as coherent, an impression on policing as schol-
ars like to think.... Nor has research led to widespread operational changes even
when it has been accepted as true” (pp. 4–5). Mastrofski (1999) emphasized that
the challenge was not only to generate more research about useful interventions
but also “to f‌igure out how to get police to do them more often” (p. 6). Weisburd
(2008) cited the continued reliance by police on random beat patrol as an example
of this gap, given the decades of research on directed patrol and problem solving at
hot spots. Lum (2009) continued by noting the lack of research in daily policing,
suggesting that better translation of research was needed in order for evidence-
based policing to be realized.
It is clear that both researchers and police innovators want research to be use-
ful and are sometimes frustrated by its lack of use. When the NRC’s Committee to
Review Research on Police Policy and Practices convened, it concluded that gaps
1
Grants.gov assignment number - NIJ-2012-3083; NIJ Solicitation number SL000978.
Available at: https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdff‌iles1/nij/sl000978.pdf.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT