Quick on the Draw: American Negativity Bias and Costly Signals in International Relations

DOI10.1177/00220027211040104
Published date01 February 2022
AuthorSeok Joon Kim
Date01 February 2022
Subject MatterArticles
2022, Vol. 66(2) 246 –271
Quick on the Draw:
American Negativity Bias
and Costly Signals in
International Relations
Seok Joon Kim
1
Abstract
States signal their intentions to domestic and foreign audiences but are not always
believed. Why do people believe some state signals but not others? Using a survey
experiment on a representative sample of the US public, this study finds that indi-
viduals have a negativity bias when assessing the credibility of state signals. They take
other states’ aggressive actions as evidence of deep hostility but are skeptical of the
credibility of conciliatory gestures. The experimental result shows that the mobili-
zation of a small proportion of an army is perceived credible enough as an aggressive
action, while the removal of even a large proportion is not perceived as conciliatory.
The psychological mechanism found here is a strong foundation for theorizing about
how individuals process information embedded in state signals and can improve our
understanding of signaling.
Keywords
negativity bias, costly signal, survey experiment, signaling perception
States signal their intentions to both domestic and foreign audiences. When North
Korea conducted several intercontinental missile tests in 2017—thereby signaling its
attainment of a nuclear deterrence to its own population as well as the international
Graduate School of International Studies (GSIS), Korea University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
Corresponding Author:
Seok Joon Kim, Graduate School of International Studies (GSIS), Korea University, 145, Anam-ro,
Seongbuk-gu, Seoul, Republic of Korea.
Emails: sjkim.ir@gmail.com; skim41@korea.ac.kr
Journal of Conflict Resolution
ªThe Author(s) 2021
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/00220027211040104
journals.sagepub.com/home/jcr
Article
Kim 247
community—the ratio of America ns perceiving North Korea as a ser ious threat
soared to 85 percent, and 50 percent of those polled favored taking military action
against the country.
1
However, outside of dramatic events such as missile testing,
people do not always believe a state’s signals. For example, Americans were divided
over the Soviet Union’s intentions near the end of the Cold War despite the Soviet
Union’s highly conciliatory gestures.
2
Similarly, Northeast Asians have never
entirely abandoned their suspicion towards Japan after World War II, despite the
country’s severe restrictions on the use of force in its pacifist constitution.
3
Why do people believe some state signals but not others? More broadly, how do
people infer other states’ intentions and make foreign policy decisions when it is not
certain whether the other is an enemy? The logic of costly signals in the international
relations (IR) literature explains when and how rational actors update their prior
beliefs and make decisions. According to this logic, rational act ors update their
beliefs about a state’s intentions when the state takes actions incurring a high cost
that a state hiding its true intentions cannot bear. This type of costly action arguably
contributed to the mollification of the British and French’s fear about their historical
rival’s (i.e., Germany) reunification in the late 1980s.
Recently, IR scholars have started paying closer attention to the microfounda-
tions of the logic of costly signals since Jervis (1970) and others developed the
concept of signals as a means of states con veying their intentions (Quek 2016 ;
Kertzer 2016; Kertzer, Renshon, and Yarhi-Milo 2021; Yarhi-Milo, Kertzer, and
Renshon 2018). They have tested whether individuals actually infer state intentions
according to the logic of costly signals, but most have focused on testing how
hostile-type signals (e.g., signals of resolve) in crisis bargaining are perceived, and
less on how benign-type signals, (e.g., signals of reassurance) or both concurrently,
are perceived with few exceptions (Kim 2016; Albuyeh and Paradis 2018).
This study examines how individuals perceive state intentions and shape foreign
policy preferences when state intentions are not clear. This study finds that costly
signals are perceived as credible only under certain conditions, showing that the
logic of costly signals in IR works differently conditional on signal types. Benign
and hostile state signals with equal strength have different impacts on individuals’
inferences and their foreign policy preferences. This paper argues that people have a
negativity bias in perceiving state intentions and making foreign policy decisions:
“(t)he psychological phenomenon that people tend to attach greater weight to neg-
ative information than to equally extreme and equally likely positive information in
a variety of information-processing tasks” (Aragones 1997, 189). In this IR context,
Americans tend to pay more attention to a state’s hostile actions than its peaceful
ones and thus weigh negative information more heavily than positive information in
assessing the credibility of state actions. This study also finds evidence showing
some properties of negativity bias including negative potency, negative differentia-
tion, and greater steepness of the negativity gradient. This study furthermore finds
that, contrary to what was expected, a motivational mechanism is not supported as
the causal logic for the negativity bias in signaling perception.
2Journal of Conflict Resolution XX(X)

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT