Putting time in perspective: How and why construal level buffers the relationship between wait time and aggressive tendencies

AuthorSandra L. Robinson,Michael A. Daniels,Dorit Efrat‐Treister
Published date01 March 2020
Date01 March 2020
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1002/job.2433
SPECIAL ISSUE ARTICLE
Putting time in perspective: How and why construal level
buffers the relationship between wait time and aggressive
tendencies
Dorit Efrat-Treister
1
| Michael A. Daniels
2
| Sandra L. Robinson
2
1
Guilford Glazer School of Business and
Management, Ben-Gurion University of the
Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
2
Sauder School of Business, The University of
British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia,
Canada
Correspondence
Dorit Efrat-Treister, Guilford Glazer School of
Business and Management, Ben-Gurion
University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva 8499000,
Israel.
Email: tdorit@bgu.ac.il
Funding information
Israel Association for Canadian Studies; Israel
Science Foundation, Grant/Award Number:
11/5322; Israeli Centers for Research
Excellence
Summary
We spend a substantial part of our daily life waiting, and unfortunately, wait time can
fuel aggressive tendencies. Our study examines the relationship between wait time,
perceived wait time, and aggressive tendencies from a construal level perspective. In
Study 1, we found that the higher the construal level, the stronger the relationship
between actual and perceived wait time and the stronger relationship between per-
ceived wait time and aggressive tendencies. In Study 2, we manipulated construal
level and found that power explains the moderating impact of construal on the wait
aggressive tendencies relationships. Results demonstrate the role of construal in
explaining both perceived wait time and aggressive responses to long wait times,
suggesting that mental construal influences both the psychological experience of
time and the subsequent reaction to that experience. Overall, these results contrib-
ute to research on subjective time perspective by enhancing the knowledge and
understanding of the determinants and effects of perceived wait time.
KEYWORDS
aggressive tendencies, construal level, power, time perception, wait time
1|INTRODUCTION
Time delays and waits at work are common and impactful (Allen,
Lehmann & Rogelberg, 2018; Guenter, Van Emmerik, & Schreurs,
2014; Rogelberg et al., 2014). They take a myriad of forms in organi-
zations (Maister, 1985), from waiting for returned calls, decisions, or
receiving critical information for task completion. A particularly com-
mon form of waiting involves meeting delays. More than half of all
meetings start late (Allen, Lehmann-Willenbrock, & Rogelberg, 2018;
Elsayed-Elkhouly, Lazarus, & Forsythe, 1997). Employees arrive late to
meetings millions of times each day and over 60% of the time; this
results in waits for the rest of the employees (Rogelberg et al., 2014).
This is especially significant given that meetings occupy a significant
portion of work time (Rogelberg, Leach, Warr, & Burnfield, 2006), and
employees increasingly report shortages of time and time stress
(Perlow, 1999; Robinson & Godbey, 1997).
Delays and waiting at work are aversive experiences (Freud,
1959), resulting in negative mood states, anger, and frustration
(Maister, 1985; Rogelberg et al., 2014; Taylor, 1994), along with ten-
dencies toward incivility or aggressive tendencies (Efrat-Treister et al.,
2019; Mroz & Allen, 2017). These findings are consistent with broader
research highlighting the relationship between wait time and aggres-
sive thoughts, feelings, and actions (Groth & Gilliland, 2006;
Munichor & Rafaeli, 2007).
Given the ubiquity and negative impact of waits and delays in
organizations, it remains relatively understudied, especially with
regard to its impact on aggressive tendencies. This is especially critical
in light of the known detrimental effect of aggressive tendencies upon
targets and work relationships (Hershcovis, 2011; Hershcovis et al.,
2007). Although the experience of waiting may be inevitable, it is
important to explore ways of reducing aggressive responses to waits.
We postulate that one way to do so is by examining the role of
Received: 16 April 2018 Revised: 17 January 2020 Accepted: 17 January 2020
DOI: 10.1002/job.2433
© 2020 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/job J Organ Behav. 2020;41:294309.
294
construal level theory (CLT) in this experience. Hence, we seek to
answer the following question: How does construal level (CL) impact
employees' perceptions of waiting and their aggressive tendencies fol-
lowing the wait?
A key to understanding and potentially altering the relationship
between wait time and aggressive tendencies comes from consider-
ation of how the perception of wait time is subjective and differs
between individuals (e.g., Eldor et al., 2017; Rousseau & Fried, 2001).
This subjectivity has two co-occurring manifestations: one, in terms of
the perception of wait time, which may vary from actual wait time
(e.g., Allman, Teki, Griffiths, & Meck, 2014) and two, in terms of the
reactions to the perceived wait time (e.g., Rafaeli, Barron, & Haber,
2002). By focusing on variables that contribute to the experience of
wait time, and explaining how wait time contributes to aggression, we
advance our understanding of the causes and effects of subjective
time perspective (see e.g., Fried & Slowik, 2004; Zimbardo & Boyd,
1999). Thus, two individuals facing the same objective wait time may
perceive and react to it differently. To the extent that we can identify
a factor that influences both the perceived wait time and the aggres-
sive reactions to it, we will be able to reduce such aggressive reactions
when waiting is inevitable
We can identify such a factor by turning our attention to CLT,
which highlights how different levels of construal explain differ-
ences in perceived time (Hansen & Trope, 2013; Trope &
Liberman, 2003; Liberman, Sagristano, & Trope, 2002). CL refers to
the level of abstraction at which one mentally represents the
world, such that lower CL involves more concrete thinking, and
higher CL involves more abstract thinking. As we argue in this
paper, CL can impact the effect of wait duration on aggressive ten-
dencies in two ways: altering perceptions of time duration and
influencing reactions to perceived wait. Those with low CL com-
pared with higher CL, perceive more markers in the passage of
time thus perceive a shorter wait duration. Moreover, as we will
argue, for a given perceived wait duration, those with high CL,
compared with low CL, will experience more aggressive tendencies
due to their higher sense of power and thus frustration with hav-
ing to wait.
We hope to make several contributions. We seek to contribute
to the body of research that has uncovered an important relation-
ship between wait time and aggressive tendencies (e.g., Angland,
Dowling, & Casey, 2014) by examining the complex role played by
CL. Building on the work of Hansen and Trope (2013), we show
that low CL can reduce the perception of the passage of time
which in turn may reduce aggressive tendencies related to waiting
by making the wait duration seem shorter. Moreover, we extend
this work by showing how for a given wait duration, high CL may
also increase aggressive tendencies, and demonstrate empirically
why this is the case. This in turn contributes to the small but
growing body of research that identifies the limitations of the posi-
tive influence of high CL in light of most research, which has
highlighted its positive consequences (e.g., Rosen, Koopman,
Gabriel, & Johnson, 2016; higher organizational learning: Reyt &
Wiesenfeld, 2015; Henderson & Trope, 2009).
This research comprises two related studies, as elaborated on
below. First, we theorize and test a model wherein CL operates as a
double moderator influencing both the relationship between wait time
and perceived wait time as well as the relationship between perceived
wait time and aggressive tendencies (Study 1). We further hypothe-
size and test whether the moderating role of CL on the latter relation-
ship can be explained by a sense of power (Study 2). See Figure 1 for
our hypothesized model.
2|THEORETICAL DEVELOPMENT AND
HYPOTHESES
Aggression is defined as behavior intended to harm others (Rippon,
2000) and includes actions that range from raising one's voice,
using foul language, threatening, throwing objects, or engaging in
sabotage. These behaviors tend to be uncivil (e.g., Andersson &
Pearson, 1999), deviant (e.g., Dunlop & Lee, 2004; Robinson &
Bennett, 1995), and retaliatory (e.g., Hershcovis, 2011). Mild forms
of aggression, such as raising one's voice often precede more
severe types of aggression such as physical abuse (Dupré & Bar-
ling, 2006); thus, it is imperative to understand triggers of aggres-
sion and curtail its occurrence. In this research, we examine
aggressive tendencies, which we define as the self-reported desire
to act with aggression. This focus is chosen given its wider occur-
rence in organizations, and because it is an important precursor to
actual aggressive behavior (Anderson & Bushman, 2001; Ham-
mock & Richardson, 1992). Thus, aggressive tendencies are impor-
tant to recognize and manage before they escalate into aggression.
A known trigger of aggressive tendencies is waiting. Wait time
often thwarts the achievement of personal goals (Rafaeli et al.,
2002) and is perceived as a waste of valuable time (Blayac &
Causse, 2001; Börjesson & Eliasson, 2014; Kawamura, 2000),
therefore eliciting frustration (e.g., Anderson, Camacho, & Bal-
krishnan, 2003; Gates, Ross, & McQueen, 2005; Pich et al., 2011;
Pruyn & Smidts, 1998) and aggressive tendencies (Berkowitz, 1989;
FIGURE 1 Full research model [Colour figure can be viewed at
wileyonlinelibrary.com]
EFRAT-TREISTER ET AL.295

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT