Putting homicide followed by suicide in context: Do macro‐environmental characteristics impact the odds of committing suicide after homicide?*

Published date01 February 2019
AuthorEmma E. Fridel,Gregory M. Zimmerman
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/1745-9125.12195
Date01 February 2019
Received: 7 February 2018 Revised: 20 July 2018 Accepted: 23 July2018
DOI: 10.1111/1745-9125.12195
ARTICLE
Putting homicide followed by suicide in context:
Do macro-environmental characteristics impact
the odds of committing suicide after homicide?*
Emma E. Fridel Gregory M. Zimmerman
School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Northeastern University
Correspondence
Gregory M. Zimmerman, PhD, Graduate
ProgramDirector and Associate Professor,
Schoolof Criminology and Cr iminal Justice,
Northeastern University,431 Churchill Hall,
360Huntington Avenue, Boston, MA 02115.
Email:g.zimmer man@northeastern.edu
Additionalsupporting information can be
foundin the listing for this article in the Wiley
OnlineLibrar y at https://onlinelibrary.wiley.
com/doi/10.1111/crim.2019.57.issue-1/
issuetoc.
Abstract
Homicide followed by suicide remains an understudied
phenomenon in the criminological literature. This is due, in
part, to methodological and statistical limitations—much of
the extant research includes small samples and has not kept
pace with quantitative advances. Moreover, scholarship on
homicide–suicide has been focused almost exclusively on
individual risk factors, discounting contextual influences.
In this study, we examine whether macro-environmental
characteristics affect the odds of suicide after a homicide.
We use data on 24,373 homicide and homicide–suicide
cases distributed across 3,019 cities and 48 U.S. states from
the National Violent Death Reporting System to examine
the direct effects of structural factors on the odds of suicide
after a homicide; and whether structural characteristics
condition the impact of the victim–offender relationship
on the odds of homicide–suicide. Hierarchical logistic
regression models indicate that macro-level concentrated
disadvantage decreases the odds of homicide–suicide.
Furthermore, concentrated disadvantage attenuates the
odds of suicide after the homicide of an intimate partner,
child, family member, or friend, relative to the killing
of a stranger. The findings reveal that researchers should
account for the context in which homicide–suicide occurs;
failure to do so may unintentionallydiscount a key correlate
34 © 2018 American Society of Criminology wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/crim Criminology. 2019;57:34–73.
FRIDEL AND ZIMMERMAN 35
of homicide–suicide and artificially inflate the effects of
the micro-environment.
KEYWORDS
concentrated disadvantage, homicide, homicide–suicide, neighborhood
effects
1INTRODUCTION
Compared with its component acts, homicide followed by suicide is an understudied phenomenon in
the criminological literature. This is due, in part, to its rarity—homicide–suicide has an estimated rate
of .22 per 100,000 U.S. persons, or 4.0 percent of all annual homicides and 1.5 percent of all annual
suicides (Large, Smith, & Nielssen, 2009; Logan et al., 2008), which are rare events themselves. The
infrequency with which homicide–suicide occurs poses several challenges.First, until now, researchers
have mainly discounted the utility of studying homicide–suicide as a distinct behavior, instead regard-
ing it as a variant of either homicide or suicide and studying it accordingly (Liem & Nieuwbeerta,
2010), despite the knowledge that homicide–suicide has a devastating impact on familiesand commu-
nities (Liem, 2010), garners national attention that can be used to shape the sociopolitical landscape
(McPhedran et al., 2015), and inspires copycat crimes (Towers, Gomez-Lievano, Khan, Mubayi, &
Castillo-Chavez, 2015). Second, and relatedly, many studies on homicide–suicide have methodological
and statistical shortcomings, including small sample sizes, low statistical power, inflated effect sizes,
overestimation of both type I and type II errors, inefficiency of model estimates, and low reproducibil-
ity of results (Button et al., 2013). Third, homicide–suicide has no formal legal definition (because
charges are not typically filed) and weak theoretical grounding on which few empirical tests are based
(Liem, 2010; Riedel, 2010). As a result, much of the extant research on homicide followedby suicide is
epidemiological in nature; has been focused on descriptions of isolated events rather than on scientific
assessment of larger sampling frames; has examined whether homicide–suicide is primarily suicide
driven, primarily homicide driven, or a different form of violence entirely; and has produced mixed
results (Eliason, 2009; Flynn et al., 2009; Liem, Postulart, & Nieuwbeerta, 2009; McPhedran et al.,
2015; Panczak et al., 2013; Reckdenwald & Simone, 2017).
Moreover, empirical studies on homicide–suicide have been conducted almost exclusively at the
individual level—theory and literature as to how the macro-sociological context impacts homicide
followed by suicide are sparse. Yet, scholars have long recognized that one avenue to advance crim-
inology as a discipline is macro–micro integration. For example, Parsons (1937), Weber (1922), and
later Coleman (1986) argued for methodological individualism (in contrast to methodological holism),
under which macro-level changesare g roundedin individual action. Relatedly, micro-level and macro-
level theories alone have historically been unable to explainmore t han 20 percent of variation in crim-
inal offending (Elliott, 1985; Muftic, 2009). Accordingly, recent calls to bridge the epistemological
divide between micro- and macro-criminology (Matsueda, 2017; Messner, 2012; Rosenfeld, 2011), as
well as laudable efforts at multilevel theorizing (Wikström, 2010), have been made. Concurrently, the
neighborhood effects literature (Sampson, 2012) and research on the person-context nexus (Baumer &
Arnio, 2012) have included multilevel statistical analysisas a way to examine the inextricable linkages
between individuals and their social contexts.
Guided by the neighborhood effects literature and research on the socio-structural correlates of
homicide (Land, McCall, & Cohen, 1990; Sampson, Morenoff, & Gannon-Rowley, 2002; Sharkey and
36 FRIDEL AND ZIMMERMAN
Faber, 2014), we examine the mechanisms through whichthe broader social context impacts homicide
followed by suicide. Our analysis is guided by two primary research questions. First, we investigate
whether macro-level concentrated disadvantage has a direct effect on the likelihood of suicide after
homicide. Grounded primarily in attribution theory (Heider, 1958), we hypothesize that pervasive vio-
lence in disadvantaged areas facilitates the external attribution of blame and desensitizes residents to
violence, thereby decreasing the guilt associated with homicide and reducing the odds of subsequent
suicide.
Second, we focus on the relevanceof the social context for victim–offender dynamics—arguably the
most prominent differentiating factor between homicide and homicide–suicide (Allen, 1983; Palmer &
Humphrey,1980; Selkin, 1976; Stack, 1997). Specifically, we examine whether the macro-environment
attenuates the salience of the victim–offender relationship for committing suicide after homicide. We
posit that the internal attribution of guilt associated with killing an intimate partner, family member,
or associate (relative to a stranger) will be reduced in disadvantaged areas with frequent violence—
where residents may be desensitized to violence, justify violence, and even tolerateviolence (Sampson
& Bartusch, 1998)—thereby reducing the odds of committing suicide after the homicide of a close
associate.
We thus contribute to the literature by formulating and testing hypotheses that link the social con-
text to the individual act of homicide–suicide, grounded in theories situated at both the micro and
macro levels. Additionally, we examine our hypotheses using data from a large sample of homicide
(N=22,960) and homicide–suicide (N=1,413) cases from the National Violent Death Reporting Sys-
tem (NVDRS), a state-based active Websurveillance system of death records established by the Centers
for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). Our sample size accommodates a test of several types of
intimate partners (spouses, ex-spouses, and partners), family members (children, parents, and other
family members), associates (friends, acquaintances, and other known persons), and strangers. Finally,
we examine contextual effects and cross-level interaction effects using three-level logistic regression
models to nest the 24,373 homicide and homicide–suicide sample cases within 3,019 cities distributed
across 48 contiguous U.S. states. Our narrative begins with a brief review of the individual-level cor-
relates of homicide followed by suicide beforetur ning to the relevance of the macro-social context for
homicide and homicide–suicide.
2CONCEPTUAL BACKGROUND
2.1 Individual-level correlates of homicide–suicide
Scholarship on the etiology of homicide–suicide has been focused almost exclusively on individual
differences and micro-environmental characteristics. In the extant literature, scholars have indicated
that homicide–suicide perpetrators are more likely than homicide perpetrators and suicide victims to
be male (Panczak et al., 2013), White (Logan et al., 2008; Wolfgang, 1958), adult (as compared with
adolescent for “homicide only” and adolescent or elderly for “suicide only”; Banks, Crandall, Sklar,
& Bauer, 2008; Liem et al., 2009), and married or separated (relative to single; Panczak et al., 2013;
Rosenbaum, 1990). In addition, homicide–suicide disproportionately involves firearms: The odds of
firearm usage are approximately 5 times greater and 11 times greater among homicide–suicides, respec-
tively,than among homicides and suicides (Carcach & Grabosky, 1998; Panczak et al., 2013). Scholars
also generally haveagreed that homicide–suicide shares a common set of risk factors with intimate part-
ner homicide and family homicide (Haines, Williams, & Lester, 2010; Heron, 2017; Manning, 2014;
Salari & Sillito, 2016). Most notably, these acts typically involve a history of interpersonal conflict

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT