The public salience of crime, 1960–2014: Age–period–cohort and time–series analyses†

AuthorLuzi Shi,Yunmei Lu,Justin T. Pickett
Date01 August 2020
Published date01 August 2020
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/1745-9125.12248
Received: 11 January 2019 Revised: 30 January 2020 Accepted: 9 April 2020
DOI: 10.1111/1745-9125.12248
ARTICLE
The public salience of crime, 1960–2014:
Age–period–cohort and time–series analyses*
Luzi Shi1Yunmei Lu2Justin T. Pickett3
1Department of Criminal Justice, Bridgewater
State University
2Department of Sociology, University at
Buffalo, SUNY
3School of Criminal Justice, University at
Albany, SUNY
Correspondence
LuziShi, Depar tment of Criminal Justice,
BridgewaterState University, 131 Summer
Street,Bridgewater, MA 02325
Email:lshi@albany.edu
Additionalsupporting information can
befound in the full text tab for this arti-
clein t he WileyOnline Library at http:
//onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/
crim.2020.58.issue-3/issuetoc.
Theaut hors wouldlike to thank Dr. Peter K.
Ennsfor sharing his data on newspaper cover-
ageof crime and Dr.Mark Ramirez for sharing
hisdat a on presidential statements about pun-
ishment.The aut hors are also grateful to the
editorsand t he anonymousreviewers for their
helpfulfeedback and comments.
Abstract
The public salience of crime has wide-ranging political and
social implications; it influences public trust in the govern-
ment and citizens’ everyday routines and interactions, and
it may affect policy responsiveness to punitive attitudes.
Identifying the sources of crime salience is thus important.
Two competing theoretical models exist: the objectivist
model and the social constructionist model. According
to the first, crime salience is a function of the crime rate.
According to the second, crime salience is a function of
media coverage and political rhetoric, and trends in crime
salience differ across population subgroups as a result of
differences in their responsiveness to elite initiatives. In
both theories, period-level effects predominate. Variation
in crime salience, however, mayalso reflect age and cohort
effects. Using data from 422,504 respondents interviewed
between 1960 and 2014, we first examine the nature
of crime salience using hierarchical age–period–cohort
(HAPC) models and then analyze period-level predictors
using first differences. We find that 1) crime salience
varies mostly at the period level; 2) crime salience trends
are parallel (cointegrated) across demographic, socioeco-
nomic, and partisan groups; and 3) crime salience trends
within every population subgroup are most consistent with
the constructionist model. The crime rate does not exert a
significant effect in any subgroup.
KEYWORDS
crime, media, most important problem, parallel publics, public opinion
568 © 2020 American Society of Criminology wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/crim Criminology.2020;58:568–593.
SHI ET AL.569
Understanding how the salience of crime to the public varies over time—that is, when and why mem-
bers of the public come to view crime as a pressing social problem—is important for at least three
reasons. First, shifts in the percentage of the public identifying crime as the country’s most impor-
tant problem (MIP) contribute to subsequent changes in aggregate support for punitive crime policies
(Ramirez, 2013), which in turn exert large effects on incarceration rates and criminal justice expen-
ditures, at both the state and federal levels (Enns, 2014, 2016). Put simply, crime salience indirectly
affects crime policy by shaping policy attitudes. Second, because issue salience interacts with aggregate
policy preferences to affect policy outcomes (Burstein, 2006; Lax & Phillips, 2012; Monroe, 1998),
punitive attitudes are likely to have larger effects on policymaking when the salience of crime to the
public is high (Zimring & Johnson, 2006). Third, beyond its policy implications, crime salience has
significant social, cultural, and political repercussions (Garland, 2001; Simon, 2007). Elevated crime
salience undermines trust in the government (Chanley, Rudolph, & Rahn, 2000). It also motivates
various forms of adaptive reactions—avoidance, precautionary, and defensive behaviors—by private
citizens to enhance their personal safety and protect familymembers, which in turn affect their everyday
interactions and social relationships (Warr, 2009; Warr & Ellison, 2000).
Two theoretical perspectives explain changes in the public salience of crime over time. According
to the first, the objectivist model, trends in crime salience reflect changes in actual rates of crim-
inal offending (Pickett, 2019). According to the second, the constructionist model, crime salience
varies because of changes in elite initiatives—specifically, changes in media crime coverage and polit-
ical rhetoric about crime (Beckett, 1997; Beckett & Sasson, 2004).1Both models indicate that crime
salience mainly varies at the period level, but their similarities stop there. Adjudicating between the
models is important because trends in crime salience play a central role in many theories of culture,
social structure, and crime control in late modern society (Beckett & Sasson, 2004; Garland, 2001;
Savelsberg, 1994; Simon, 2007).
Unfortunately, the few empirical studies that included aggregate data to analyze changes over time
in the public salience of crime had notable limitations. Either the researchers only reported bivariate
findings (Miller, 2013, 2016), evaluated short time periods using data from only one or two polling
organizations (Beckett, 1997; Lowry,Nio, & Leitner, 2003), or overlooked the implications of nonsta-
tionarity for time-series analysis (Hill, Oliver, & Marion, 2010; Oliver, 1998, 2002). None, as far as
we know, used multilevel data to examine simultaneously the independent effects of age, birth cohort,
and time period on perceived crime salience in the United States. This is important because “individ-
ual aging, historical contexts, and generational membership are simultaneously related to the passage
of time but have separate effects” (Gray, Grasso, Farrall, Jennings, & Hay, 2018, p. 2). As Anderson,
Lytle, and Schwadel (2017, p. 835) noted, “[S]ome social attitudes change across generations or birth
cohorts rather than across time periods.”
Additionally, according to the constructionist approach, elite initiatives are race coded and should
have larger effects among individuals who harbor anti-minority sentiments that increase their sensi-
tivity to racial “dog whistles” (Drakulich, 2015); crime salience trends should thus differ across racial
and political groups (Beckett & Sasson, 2004; Tonry,2011). Cr ime salience should also diverge along
socioeconomic cleavages. According to Garland (2001, p. 152), forexample, the “professional middle
classes” were “the dog that did not bark” during the U.S. imprisonment binge because they experienced
especially large increases in crime salience. According to the objectivist approach, on the other hand,
crime salience trends are parallel across different racial, political, and socioeconomic groups because
they all respond to the crime rate (Page & Shapiro, 1992; Pickett, 2019).
1As used herein, the term “elite initiatives” refers to agenda-setting activities by either media or political elites (Beckett &
Sasson, 2004; Chomsky & Herman, 1988).

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT