Proactive Criminal Thinking and Deviant Identity as Mediators of the Peer Influence Effect

Date01 July 2017
Published date01 July 2017
AuthorGlenn D. Walters
DOI10.1177/1541204016636436
Subject MatterArticles
Article
Proactive Criminal Thinking
and Deviant Identity as
Mediators of the Peer
Influence Effect
Glenn D. Walters
1
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to test the moral model of criminal lifestyle development with data
from the 1,725-member (918 boys and 807 girls) National Youth Survey. It was hypothesized that
peer delinquency would predict proactive criminal thinking but not deviant identity as part of a four-
variable chain running from peer delinquency to participant delinquency. Consistent with this
hypothesis, the pathway running from peer delinquency to proactive criminal thinking to deviant
identity to participant delinquency was significant but the pathway running from peer delinquency to
deviant identity to proactive criminal thinking to participant delinquency was not. Deviant identity
nonetheless predicted proactive criminal thinking and delinquency. These results support a major
pathway in the moral model and indicate that while deviant identity plays a role in antisocial
development, it is as a cause and effect of proactive criminal thinking rather than as an effect of
delinquent peer associations.
Keywords
delinquent peer associations, proactive criminal thinking, deviant identity, longitudinal, criminal
pathways
Criminological theory is a rich source of hypotheses on criminal behavior. In an effort to capitalize
on this, Walters (2016c) recently introduced an integrated theory of criminal behavior that rests
heavily on existing criminological theory—social learning theory (Akers, 1998; Sutherland, 1947),
general strain theory (Agnew, 1992), labeling theory (Becker, 1963; Lemert, 1951), and the general
theory of crime (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990), in particular. The integrated theory also shares a
great deal in common with life-course developmental theories of crime (Farrington, 2010) through
its emphasis on pathways to delinquency. In presenting his theory, Walters (2016c) emphasized
two interrelated and overlapping models, referred to as the control and moral models of criminal
lifestyle development. Both models highlight constructs from major criminological theories. The
control model, for instance, features low self-control (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990), general strain
1
Department of Criminal Justice, Kutztown University, Kutztown, PA, USA
Corresponding Author:
Glenn D. Walters, Department of Criminal Justice, Kutztown University, Kutztown, PA 19530, USA.
Email: walters@kutztown.edu
Youth Violence and JuvenileJustice
2017, Vol. 15(3) 281-298
ªThe Author(s) 2016
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1541204016636436
journals.sagepub.com/home/yvj
(Agnew, 1992), and the peer selection effect (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990), with reactive (impul-
sive, irresponsible, and emotional) criminal thinking serving as a principal mediator of pathways
linking these constructs. Walters (2015a) recently tested several of the control model pathways and
discovered that low self-contr ol led to peer rejection (general s train) and peer rejection led to
delinquent peer associations by way of both delinquency (peer selection effect) and reactive criminal
thinking.
Complementing the control model is a moral model of criminal lifestyle development that
features callous/unemotional traits instead of low self-control as the core construct and includes
labeling and identify transformation (Lemert, 1951), cumulative disadvantage (Sampson & Laub,
1993), and peer influence (Sutherland, 1947) as central concepts and proactive (planned, calculated,
and unemotional) criminal thinking as the principal mediator of relationships. Callous/unemotional
traits were originally derived from Facet 2 of Hare’s (2003) Psychopathy Checklist–Revised
(PCL-R) but have since been studied in juvenile populations by Frick and colleagues (Frick, Ray,
Thompson, & Kahn, 2014; Roose, Bijttebier, Decoene, Claes, & Frick, 2010). Research indicates
that compared to children and adolescents with below average to average scores on measures of
callous/unemotional traits, those who score high on these traits are more involved in serious crime
(Frick et al., 2014). Children who exhibit callous/unemotional traits are also more likely to offend in
groups (Goldweber, Dmitrieva, Ca uffman, Piquero, & Steinberg, 2011 ; Thornton et al., 2015),
affiliate with gangs (Thornton et al., 2015), assume leadership roles in a criminal group or gang
(Kerr, Van Zalk, & Stattin, 2012; Thornton et al., 2015), and engage in offenses that are planned and
premeditated (Lawing, Frick, & Cruise, 2010). These findings fit well with the strategic and calcu-
lated nature of proactive criminal thinking and instrumental criminal behavior.
The moral model of criminal lifestyle development is shown in Figure 1. Several of the individual
pathways in this model have already received preliminary support. The causal relationship presumed
to exist between callous/unemotional traits, as measured by Factor 1 of the PCL: Youth Version
(Forth, Kosson, & Hare, 2003), and violent crime was found to be mediated by proactive criminal
thinking in a study by Walters and DeLisi (2015). This would appear to confirm the lower front
pathway in Figure 1 running from callous/unemotional traits to proactive criminal thinking to
delinquency. The connections between callous/unemotional traits and delinquency (Frick et al.,
2014) and between delinquency and delinquent peer associations (Kiesner, Poulin, & Nicotra,
2003) are well established, thus supporting the upper front pathways depicted in Figure 1. There
is also substantial support for the lower middle pathways running from delinquency to labeling to
delinquency and from delinquency to labeling to identity transformation (Bernburg & Krohn, 2003).
Less is known about the upper middle portion of the model, particularly the pathway running from
delinquent peer associations to proactive criminal thinking to identity transformation to delin-
quency, although even here there is evidence that neutralization techniques, which can be considered
a feature of proactive criminal thinking, are learned from friends (Maruna & Copes, 2005).
Research on the consequences of delinquent peer associations provides consistent support for the
peer socialization or influence effect that runs from deviant peer associations to delinquency
(Gifford-Smith, Dodge, Dishion, & McCord, 2005). It has traditionally been assumed that the peer
influence effect is mediated by adolescent involvement in unstructured and unsupervised routine
activities (Osgood, Wilson, O’Malley, Bachman, & Johnston, 1996), although several recent studies
have shown that proactive criminal thinking is also capable of mediating the peer influence effect
(Megens & Weerman, 2012; Walters, 2015b, 2016a). Walters (2016b), in fact, determined that
without cognitive mediation, the peer influence effect failed to materialize. An important research
question, then, is whether additional variables play a role in mediating the peer influence effect
given that multiple mediator models sometimes do a better job of modeling the real world than single
mediator models. Mediators in a multiple mediator model are aligned parallel to one another or
serially. Parallel multiple mediation is where two or more variables simultaneously mediate the
282 Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice 15(3)

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT