Predicting a Partner's End‐of‐Life Preferences, or Substituting One's Own?

DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12030
Date01 June 2013
AuthorSara M. Moorman,Megumi Inoue
Published date01 June 2013
SARA M. MOORMAN Boston College
MEGUMI INOUE Boston College
Predicting a Partner’s End-of-Life Preferences,
or Substituting One’s Own?
Numerous studies comparing patients’ end-
of-life health care treatment preferences with
their surrogates’ reports of those preferences
indicate that partners do not know one another’s
treatment preferences: Random guesses are
just as likely as surrogate choices to match
the patients’ preferences. The present study
uses the empathic accuracy model and the
assumed similarity model to shed further light
on accuracy and bias in surrogate reports.
The data come from the National Center
for Family and Marriage Research/Knowledge
Networks Pilot Study 2010, a national sample
of 1,075 heterosexual American couples age 18
to 64. Surrogate reports were simultaneously
biased (i.e., correlated with the surrogate’s
own preference) and accurate (i.e., correlated
with the partner’s actual preference). Higher
relationship quality and legal marriage (vs.
cohabitation) were associated with increased
assumed similarity bias. These f‌indings suggest
that practitioners working in end-of-life care
need to be familiar with their partnered patients’
relationship dynamics.
Department of Sociology and Institute on Aging, Boston
College, 140 Commonwealth Ave., McGuinn Hall 426,
Chestnut Hill, MA 02467-3807 (moormans@bc.edu).
School of Social Work and Institute on Aging, Boston
College, 140 Commonwealth Ave. McGuinn Hall, Chestnut
Hill, MA 02467.
Key Words: dyadic/couple data, families in middle and later
life, health, multilevel models, relationship satisfaction, U.S.
families.
Throughout the life course, romantic partners
monitor one another’s health, providing pressure
to adopt healthy behaviors, encouragement to
adhere to those behaviors, and instrumental
assistance in doing so (Thoits, 2011). Partners
continue affecting one another’s health even
at the end of life, when a partner can be
instrumental in determining the health care a
dying person receives. In 70% of cases in
which a dying older person requires a treatment
decision, the person has lost the capacity to make
decisions for him- or herself (Silveira, Kim, &
Langa, 2010). For 75% of married older adults,
the spouse serves as the legal surrogate who is
authorized to make medical decisions on behalf
of the incapacitated patient (known as a durable
power of attorney for health care [DPAHC];
Carr & Khodyakov, 2007).
The law instructs surrogates to apply the
standard of substituted judgment, that is, to
choose the treatment that the patient him-
or herself would choose if able (Sabatino,
2010). Nevertheless, numerous studies that
have used hypothetical treatment scenarios to
match participants’ preferences to their spousal
surrogates’ reports of those preferences indicate
that spouses may often fail at substituted
judgment: Random guesses are just as likely
as surrogate choices to match participants’
preferences (e.g., Moorman & Carr, 2008;
Shalowitz, Garrett-Meyer, & Wendler, 2006).
Furthermore, although many studies have
investigated factors that may inf‌luence sub-
stituted judgment, few have identif‌ied reliable
correlates. For instance, men and women are
734 Journal of Marriage and Family 75 (June 2013): 734 –745
DOI:10.1111/jomf.12030

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT