Perceived arrest risk, psychic rewards, and offense specialization: A partial test of rational choice theory†

AuthorKyle J. Thomas,Benjamin C. Hamilton,Thomas A. Loughran
Date01 August 2020
Published date01 August 2020
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/1745-9125.12243
Received: 26 June 2019 Revised: 27 February 2020 Accepted: 27 February 2020
DOI: 10.1111/1745-9125.12243
ARTICLE
Perceived arrest risk, psychic rewards, and offense
specialization: A partial test of rational choice
theory*
Kyle J. Thomas1Thomas A. Loughran2Benjamin C. Hamilton3
1University of Colorado—Boulder
2The Pennsylvania State University
3University of Missouri—St. Louis
Correspondence
KyleJ. Thomas, Department of Sociology,
Universityof Colorado Boulder Ketchum Arts
andSciences, Boulder, CO 80309.
Email:kyle.thomas@colorado.edu
Additionalsupporting information
canbe found in the full text tab for this
article in the WileyOnline Librar y at
http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/
crim.2020.58.issue-3/issuetoc.
Wewould like to thank Dan Nagin, Holly
Nguyen,the anonymous reviewers and, the
Editor,David McDowall for their helpful feed-
backon earlier versions of this article.
Abstract
In prior tests of Beckerian rational choice theory, the notion
that individuals are responsive to the (dis)incentives associ-
ated with crime has been supported. Much of this research
has comprised composite scores of perceived rewards and
risks of multiple, often disparate, crime types that are then
used to predict “general” offending behavior. Although the
results of such prior tests are informative, we believe that
this tendency has resulted in two shortcomings. First, a
central component of mathematical rational choice the-
ory is overlooked, namely, that responsivity to incentives
will be crime specific. That is, offenders should prefer
crime types that subjectively offer greater rewards and
fewer risks relative to other crimes. Second, individual dif-
ferences in offending specialization are not addressed, of
which Clarke and Cornish (1985) and Shover(1996) argued
rational choice theories are well suited to explain. Using a
sample of serious offenders, we find that in a given time
period, individuals are more likely to engage in crime types
they viewed as more intrinsically rewarding and less risky
compared with other crimes. Furthermore, individuals dis-
played greater specialization in violence to the extent they
view violence as more rewarding and less risky than prop-
erty offenses
KEYWORDS
offender decision-making, rational choice theory, specialization
Criminology. 2020;58:485–509. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/crim © 2020 American Society of Criminology 485
486 THOMAS ET AL.
The rational choice perspective in criminology differs from other traditions in its explicit focus on
the decision-making processes underlying offending (Becker,1968; McCart hy, 2002; see also Hechter
& Kanazawa, 1997). Although numerous variations of rational choice theories exist (Becker, 1968;
Cornish & Clarke, 1986; McCarthy, 2002), these perspectives all share the common assumption that
individuals are responsive to incentives. Most quantitativetests have consisted of the formal mathemat-
ical interpretation grounded in the traditional expected utility (von Neumann & Morgenstern, 1945)
model originally proposed by Becker (1968), including constituent elements suchas Bayesian updating
(Anwar & Loughran, 2011; Matsueda, Kreager, & Huizinga, 2006), and have included a regression of
self-reported offending on rational choice inputs to examine whether the risks and rewards associated
with crime predict offending (e.g., Loughran, Paternoster, Chalfin, & Wilson, 2016). The findings
from extant research show support for rational choice predictions, suggesting that potential offend-
ers consider the consequences associated with crime when deciding whether to offend (see Loughran
et al., 2016; Matsueda et al., 2006; Nagin, 2007; Paternoster, 2010). Much of this research has utilized
composite scores of risk and rewards to predict variation in a general offending outcome (Paternoster,
1986; Piliavin, Gartner, Thornton, & Matsueda, 1986). That is, key constructs like perceived arrest
risk and psychic rewards are typically measured as an aggregation of subjective perceptions across
multiple, often disparate crime types,1which are then linked to aggregate offending. The implication
is that monolithic measurements of “risk” and “rewards” are general, rather than crime-specific factors
in offending decisions. As Shover (1996, p. 65) noted, however, “the existence of distinct crime pref-
erences [tends] to draw [individuals] back to the type of crime they feel most at home with.” In
other words, individuals hold crime-specific perceptions and preferences that make them more likely
to commit certain types of crime relative to others.2
The findings from prior research has shown that many individuals view some crimes as thrilling
and exciting but see other types of crime as eliciting little in terms of intrinsic rewards (Katz, 1988;
Shover, 1996; Steffensmeier, 1986; Steffensmeier & Ulmer, 2005; Topalli, 2006; Wright & Decker,
1997). Furthermore, Anwar and Loughran (2011) found that perceived arrest risk was updated in a
nuanced manner within persons across different types of crime and highlighted that individuals tend
to view some crimes as less risky than others (see also Thomas, Hamilton, & Loughran, 2018). To
the extent that individuals are responsive to the risks and rewards associated with offending, scholars
drawing on the rational choice perspective have explicitly suggested that subjective expectations of
different crimes should vary within persons (Nagin, 1998) and, thus, affect the qualitative nature of
individuals’ offending by allowing fordifferent choice calculi for different crimes (i.e., an individual’s
tendency to commit specific crimes over others; Anwar & Loughran, 2011; Cornish & Clarke, 1985;
Matsueda et al., 2006; Shover, 1996). Todate, however, little research has been conducted to examine
this prediction directly (Nagin, 1998).
The notion that the types of crimes3individuals commit may be a function of within-person variation
in the risks and rewards associated with crime has several implications. First, when keeping perceived
1For example, it is common for authors to report a Cronbach’s alpha combining all crimes for these types of measures (see
https://www.pathwaysstudy.pitt.edu/codebook/personal-and-social-costs-and-rewards-sb.html).
2Others have openly questioned the assumption that perceptions are global as opposed to crime specific (Nagin, 1998).
3We use the terms “crime-specific,” “crime types,” and “types of crime” to reflect both specific criminal acts (e.g., fighting
and theft) as well as broader offense categories such as violent and property crimes. Both can be contrasted to a general overall
offendingmeasure. Alt hough at times this mayseem confusing, there is no consensus in t he field as to the appropriate granularity
when assessing qualitative dimensions of offending(Mazerolle, Brame, Paternoster, Piquero, & Dean, 2000; Sullivan, McGloin,
Pratt, & Piquero, 2006). Even rational choicet heorists havediscussed both specific acts and broader crime-type categor ies when
considering crime-specific effects, and both outcome operationalizations are consistent with a rational choice approach.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT