Party–Group Ambivalence and Voter Loyalty: Results From Three Experiments

AuthorGregory Shufeldt
DOI10.1177/1532673X17705854
Published date01 January 2018
Date01 January 2018
Subject MatterArticles
https://doi.org/10.1177/1532673X17705854
American Politics Research
2018, Vol. 46(1) 132 –168
© The Author(s) 2017
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1532673X17705854
journals.sagepub.com/home/apr
Article
Party–Group
Ambivalence and Voter
Loyalty: Results From
Three Experiments
Gregory Shufeldt1
Abstract
Renewed emphasis on the group-based nature of political parties makes
understanding the relationship between partisan and group identities
essential. How do citizens respond to the internal disconnect between their
partisan identity and their other politically salient identities? In addition,
do differences in the group-based nature of each party lead to asymmetric
effects of party–group ambivalence? Using data from an original survey
experiment across three samples—the 2012 Cooperative Congressional
Election Study, the 2012 Convention Delegate Study, and a 2015 sample from
Amazon Mechanical Turk—I find that party–group ambivalence diminishes
party loyalty, making respondents less likely to vote for, contribute to, or
volunteer for their political party’s candidate. Moreover, the strength of this
impact is consistently larger among Republican identifiers than Democratic
identifiers. These results suggest that party asymmetry in party coalitions
may have an impact on party loyalty.
Keywords
ambivalence, political parties, voting behavior
1Butler University, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Corresponding Author:
Gregory Shufeldt, Butler University, 4600 Sunset Ave., Jordan Hall 345, Indianapolis,
IN 46208, USA.
Email: gshufeld@butler.edu
705854APRXXX10.1177/1532673X17705854American Politics ResearchShufeldt
research-article2017
Shufeldt 133
The group-based nature of political parties makes understanding the relation-
ship between party and group essential. Yet, as renewed emphasis is placed
on the group-based nature of political parties, there remains a need to test
how citizens respond to ambivalence between their partisan identity and
other nonpartisan, politically salient identities, such as ideology or social
identity. How does partygroup ambivalence, a disconnect between party
and group evaluations, affect partisan political behavior?
Moreover, recent research has pointed to asymmetries between the parties
in the role that groups play (Shufeldt, 2014; Grossman & Hopkins, 2015,
2016). If Democrats are a party that promotes group benefits while
Republicans promote ideological goals, it stands to reason that party–group
ambivalence may affect the parties differently. The standard conventional
wisdom has long posited Democrats to be the disorganized, fractured party
with Republicans a more disciplined, homogeneous party. Recent Republican
turmoil is particularly newsworthy because it flies in the face of what we
have come to expect. Paul Waldman (2014) wrote in The American Prospect:
For the longest time, Democrats were the party of infighting and disunity,
whose squabbling never failed to find its way into the news. It’s a grim inside
joke among liberals that the most common headline in the political media is
“Democrats in Disarray.” But it hasn’t been that way for a while. In fact,
perhaps the most important political dynamic of the current era is the conflict
within the previously monolithic Republican party.
To test whether party–group ambivalence has asymmetric effects for
Democratic and Republican identifiers, I conducted an original experiment
across three samples: the 2012 Cooperative Congressional Election Study
(CCES), the 2012 Convention Delegate Study (CDS), and a 2015 sample
recruited from Amazon Mechanical Turk (mTurk). In the experiment, voters
are exposed to fictitious candidate vignettes where the endorsement of their
political party and political groups most important to them are varied ran-
domly. Respondents then are asked about their likelihood of voting for, con-
tributing to, and volunteering for, either candidate.
The findings suggest that the relationship between partisan and group iden-
tities can strengthen party loyalty. Reinforcing cues maximize support as
respondents demonstrate more party loyalty by becoming more likely to vote
for, contribute to, and volunteer for their party. However, when voters experi-
ence ambivalence between their partisanship and another politically salient
group identity, partisan support is diminished. Explicit conflict between cues
and the absence of reinforcing cues have a more pronounced effect among
Republican identifiers—diminishing party loyalty. Taken together, these results
134 American Politics Research 46(1)
suggest higher rates of party–group ambivalence may dampen any perceived
advantage Republicans might have once enjoyed in party loyalty. This provides
support for the idea that Republican appeals based on broad ideological goals
at the expense of emphasizing group benefits is a sound electoral strategy.
Party–Group Ambivalence and Partisan Behavior
Our political identities are the product of multiple competing identities. As peo-
ple identify with these various groups, they form an in-group versus out-group
bias, or more simply an “us versus them” bias (Kinder & Kam, 2009; Tajfel &
Turner, 1979; Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987). As competi-
tion between the parties grows fiercer, this us versus them dynamic drives higher
levels of polarization, animosity, and bias directed at members of the opposing
party (Iyengar, Sood, & Lelkes, 2012; Iyengar & Westwood, 2015). In spite of
the strength of party identification, many voters still are able to identify both
positive and negative aspects of their party (Basinger & Lavine, 2005). Voters
are frequently ambivalent, defined as “an individual’s endorsement of compet-
ing considerations relevant to evaluating an attitude object” (Lavine, 2001, p.
915), between the parties. More specifically, partisan ambivalence is defined as
a disconnect between party identification and party evaluations (Lavine,
Johnston, & Steenbergen, 2012). Ambivalent partisans are the voters who expe-
rience this disconnect. Univalent partisans are voters who do not experience
ambivalence between the parties. More recent studies in political psychology
have sought to bring attention back to ambivalence (e.g., Greene, 2005;
Groenendyk, 2013; Lavine et al., 2012), arguing that ambivalence has a clear
effect on political attitudes (Alvarez & Brehm, 2002) and political behavior
(Basinger & Lavine, 2005; see Thornton, 2013, for a recent summary).1
Here is where the work of Liliana Mason and colleagues has been most
illuminating. Her work on political identity alignment has found that “cor-
rectly sorted” partisans are more likely to be motivated to engage in political
participation, will sense larger differences between the parties, and have
lower levels of tolerance than partisans who are incorrectly sorted (Huddy,
Mason, & Aaroe, 2015; Huddy, Mason, & Horwitz, 2016; L. Mason, 2013,
2015, 2016). When identities are correctly aligned, in-group bias and out-
group intolerance are heightened (Roccas & Brewer, 2002). As a result,
stronger partisan identifiers are driven to participate via enthusiasm for their
own groups and anger directed at opposing groups (Groenendyk & Banks,
2014; P. R. Miller & Conover, 2015). This body of work largely confirms the
early research finding of how voters respond to cross-pressure (Berelson,
Lazarsfeld, & McPhee, 1954; A. Campbell, Converse, Miller, & Stokes,
1960).

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT