Parenting Style and Adolescent Externalizing Behaviors: The Moderating Role of Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia

Published date01 August 2016
AuthorRandal D. Day,Daniel J. Blocker,W. Justin Dyer,Roy A. Bean
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12316
Date01 August 2016
W. J D Brigham Young University
D J. B Greenville, NC
R D. D Brigham Young University∗∗
R A. B Brigham Young University∗∗∗
Parenting Style and Adolescent Externalizing
Behaviors: The Moderating Role of Respiratory
Sinus Arrhythmia
This longitudinal study was conducted with 262
adolescents (M age =15.3) and their parents to
examine adolescent respiratory sinus arrhyth-
mia (RSA; baseline and reactivity) as a moder-
ator of the relationship between parenting style
and adolescent externalizing behaviors. This
was conceptualized within the differential sus-
ceptibility to the environment framework, which
proposes that children with greater sensitivity
to environmental inuences benet more from
supportive environments but are at greater risk
in averse environments. In this study, low RSA
baseline was associated with greater suscepti-
bility. In conrmation of hypotheses, males with
low RSA baseline had the most externalizing
behaviors when mother or father authoritative
Religious Education, Family Education, 270N JSB,
Brigham YoungUniversity, Provo, UT 84602
(wjd@byu.edu).
306-G Horseshoe Dr., Greenville, NC 27834.
∗∗Professor Emeritus, School of Family Life, Brigham
YoungUniversity, Provo, UT 84602.
∗∗∗School of Family Life, 258 TLRB, Brigham Young
University,Provo, UT 84602.
KeyWords: adolescents, differential susceptibility,parenting
style, respiratory sinus arrhythmia.
parenting was low or when mother authoritar-
ian parenting was high. Contrary to hypothe-
ses, females with greater RSA reactivity (high
susceptibility) did more poorly when authoritar-
ian parenting was low or authoritative parenting
was high. Differential gender socialization and
the task used to elicit RSA reactivity are sug-
gested reasons for gender differences.
Recent theoretical and empirical work suggests
that children who are more sensitive to the envi-
ronment will thrive in supportive environments
but will struggle in adverse ones. Conversely,
children who are less sensitive to the environ-
ment do not benet as much from supportive
environments but also do not struggle as much
in aversive environments (Ellis & Boyce, 2011;
Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg,
& Van IJzendoorn, 2011). Some have used the
“orchid” and “dandelion” metaphor to describe
this (Boyce & Ellis, 2005); that is, “orchid”
children (i.e., sensitive) ourish in highly
enriching conditions but struggle in adverse
conditions, whereas “dandelion” children (i.e.,
less sensitive) tend to ourish under a variety of
conditions.
This phenomenon is often referred to as dif-
ferential susceptibility to the environment(DSE;
Journal of Marriage and Family 78 (August 2016): 1149–1165 1149
DOI:10.1111/jomf.12316
1150 Journal of Marriage and Family
Ellis, Boyce, et al., 2011). Empirical support for
this can been found across a variety of indicators
of child sensitivity (e.g., temperament, genetic
type, neurophysiologic functioning) as well
a variety of measures of environmental sup-
port and stress (e.g., levels of marital conict,
positive and negative parenting, life stresses;
Bakermans-Kranenburg & van IJzendoorn,
2011; Eisenberg et al., 2012; Obradovi´
c, Bush,
Stamperdahl, Adler, & Boyce, 2010). However,
within this growing research area no studies
have yet examined whether DSE also applies to
the basic parenting dimensions of authoritative
and authoritarian parenting. Although some
studies have examined DSE hypotheses with
other, similar, parenting constructs (Mesman
et al., 2009; Morris et al., 2002; Simons et al.,
2013), the conceptual framework introduced
by Baumrind (1966) has yet to be explicitly
studied with DSE propositions (for inciden-
tal connections, see Ellis, Shirtcliff, Boyce,
Deardorff, & Essex, 2011, and Morris, Cui,
& Steinberg, 2013). This is unfortunate, as
Baumrind’s framework is “widely recognized as
the leading typological approach to parenting”
(Criss & Larzelere, 2013, p. 5) and frequently
is used in the family and parenting literatures.
By explicitly linking the DSE and parenting
styles, both areas will have additional concep-
tual frameworks to draw on in building models
of how parenting inuences child development.
In the current study we investigated whether
the supportive environment of authoritative
parenting beneted high-sensitivity children
more than their low-sensitivity counterparts. We
further examined whether the high-sensitivity
children struggled more under the aversive,
authoritarian parenting style. Within this we
also considered whether the gender of the child
and parent moderates these associations; that is,
we examined whether the inuence of a child’s
sensitivity depends partly on whether the child
is male or female and whether the parent is the
mother or the father.
As with several other DSE studies, parasym-
pathetic nervous system (PNS) activity (both
baseline and reactivity levels) was used as an
index of sensitivity (Belsky & Pluess, 2009;
Kennedy, Rubin, Hastings, & Maisel, 2004;
Obradovi´
c et al., 2010). PNS activity is ideal
for indexing sensitivity because it has a substan-
tial theoretical base regarding its role in human
development (Porges, 1995, 2011). Child exter-
nalizing behaviors were used as the index of
well-being given that literatures on both author-
itative/authoritarian styles and DSE (including
those using PNS activity as the index of suscep-
tibility) have had a strong focus on this outcome.
However,as of yet no research has combined the
parenting styles literature with the DSE litera-
ture in examining externalizing behaviors.
C E B
 P S
Externalizing behaviors are dened as “overt
behaviors that have a negative effect on the
external environment” and can include behav-
iors that are disruptive, hyperactive/impulsive,
and aggressive (White & Renk, 2012). A wealth
of literature has examined numerous correlates
of externalizing behaviors (e.g., Leventhal &
Brooks-Gunn, 2000; Meunier et al., 2011), par-
enting being the most studied correlate and
central to the majority of explanatory models
(Baumrind, 1991; Darling & Steinberg, 1993;
Garcia & Garcia, 2009; Steinberg, 2001).
Baumrind’s (1966, 1967) typology is one of
the most inuential models of parenting style to
date. This typology is based on two dimensions:
(a) parental responsiveness and (b) parental
demandingness. Of interest to us in the current
study were the authoritative and authoritarian
parenting types (i.e., parenting “styles”), which
are at opposite extremes of responsiveness but
are both high on demandingness. Responsive-
ness refers to parents’ emotional warmth and
supportive actions that are attuned to “children’s
vulnerabilities, cognitions, and inputs and
supportive of children’s individual needs and
plans” (Baumrind, 2013, p. 26). Authoritative
parents are high in responsiveness, whereas
authoritarian parents are low.
Demandingness refers to monitoring, limit
setting, and having consequences for viola-
tions, and it is sometimes simply referred to as
“control” (Barber & Xia, 2013). Although
Baumrind’s typology places both authoritative
and authoritarian parents at the high end of
control, it has been argued that authoritative
and authoritarian parents differ in their methods
used to exert control. Authoritarian parents use
more psychological control, whereas author-
itative parents use more appropriate levels of
behavioral control.
Steinberg and Barber dened psychological
control as attacks on the child’s self by violating
the autonomy of the child, being excessively

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT