Nonmaternal Care's Association With Mother's Parenting Sensitivity: A Case of Self‐Selection Bias?

AuthorAlfred DeMaris,Kei M. Nomaguchi
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12022
Date01 June 2013
Published date01 June 2013
KEI M. NOMAGUCHI AND ALFRED DEMARIS Bowling Green State University
Nonmaternal Care’s Association With Mother’s
Parenting Sensitivity: A Case of Self-Selection Bias?
Although attachment theory posits that the
use of nonmaternal care undermines quality
of mothers’ parenting, empirical evidence for
this link is inconclusive. Using data from the
National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development Study of Early Child Care and
Youth Development ( N=1,233), the authors
examined the associations between nonmaternal
care characteristics and maternal sensitivity
during the f‌irst 3 years of children’s lives,
with special attention to selection effects and
moderation by resource levels. Findings from
f‌ixed-effects regression models suggested that,
on average, there is little relationship between
nonmaternal care characteristics and maternal
sensitivity, once selection factors are held
constant. Some evidence of moderation effects
was found, however. Excellent-quality care is
related to more sensitivity for mothers with lower
family income. Poor-quality care is related to
lower sensitivity for single mothers, but not
partnered mothers. In sum, nonmaternal care
characteristics do not seem to have as much
inf‌luence on mothers’ parenting as attachment
theory claims.
Between 1975 and 1998, the labor force
participation rate among mothers with children
Department of Sociology, Bowling Green State University,
Bowling Green, OH 43403 (knomagu@bgsu.edu).
Key Words: child-care arrangements, f‌ixed-effects models,
marital status, parenting styles, selection effects, socioeco-
nomic status.
under age 3 increased from 34.3% to 62.2%.
Since the peak in 1998, there has been no
clear trend (U.S. Department of Labor, Bureau
of Labor Statistics, 2010). The increase in
maternal employment during the f‌irst 3 years of
children’s lives has led to scientif‌ic and public
debates about how early nonmaternal care might
inf‌luence the mother child relationship and
the development of children (National Institute
of Child Health and Human Development
Early Child Care Research Network [NICHD
ECCRN], 2005). Although most research has
examined the direct link between nonmaternal
care and child outcomes, some researchers
speculate that the extensive use of nonmaternal
care may result in poorer child outcomes in
part indirectly through undermining mothers’
sensitive parenting (Belsky, 1999; Brazelton,
1986). Mothers’ sensitive parenting generally
refers to the extent to which mothers respond
to their children’s needs promptly, remain
patient with their children, provide adequate
stimulation, and engage in playful interactions
(Belsky, 1999). Given that mothers’ sensitive
parenting is a key factor in shaping child
outcomes (NICHD ECCRN, 2001, 2003), this
speculation is important to explore, yet relatively
little is known about how nonmaternal care may
inf‌luence the degree to which mothers exhibit
sensitive parenting.
Attachment theory has led to the popular
premise that daily separations from their chil-
dren due to the extensive use of nonmaternal care
may result in less sensitivity in mothers’ parent-
ing (Belsky, 1999, 2001). Some researchers,
760 Journal of Marriage and Family 75 (June 2013): 760 –777
DOI:10.1111/jomf.12022
Nonmaternal Care and Maternal Sensitivity 761
however, have questioned the universality of
this link, emphasizing variations by contexts of
mothers’ lives (Clark, Hyde, Essex, & Klein,
1997). Furthermore, other researchers are skep-
tical about nonmaternal care having much inf‌lu-
ence on mothers’ parenting quality, suggesting
the possibility of a selection effect; that is,
maternal sensitivity and choices made concern-
ing nonmaternal care might both be dependent
on unobserved characteristics, such as moth-
ers’ child rearing orientation (NICHD ECCRN,
1999).
The purpose of the present research was to
address this debate. Using longitudinal data from
NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth
Development (SECCYD), we reexamined the
association between nonmaternal care character-
istics and maternal sensitivity. To address vari-
ations by context, we used a stress resource
perspective and examined mothers’ resources,
such as family income and marital status, as
moderators of the association. To address the
selection issue, we used f‌ixed-effects models
that control for unmeasured characteristics that
could confound the observed association, and we
compared the results with those from random-
effects models. This study extends prior work in
that it is one of the f‌irst studies to (a) explicitly
use the stress resource perspective in linking
nonmaternal care to maternal sensitivity and (b)
attempt to rule out self-selection bias.
BACKGROUND
Prior Research
Attachment theory (Ainsworth, 1989) under-
girds the premise that the use of nonmaternal
care during the f‌irst 3 years of children’s lives is
related to poorer quality of mothers’ parenting.
It posits that extended hours of daily separations
from their children may place constraints on
mothers’ ability to learn their children’s subtle
cues, which in turn results in a failure to pro-
vide sensitive parenting (Belsky, 1999, 2001).
Although a few empirical studies have found a
link between the amount of nonmaternal care and
less sensitivity in mothers’ parenting (Belsky,
1999; NICHD ECCRN, 1999), most studies have
not. One study found that the amount of nonma-
ternal care was related to less sensitive parenting
only for mothers with depressive symptoms and
mothers whose child had a diff‌icult temperament
(Clark et al., 1997). A few studies found no
relationships between the use or amount of non-
maternal care and maternal sensitivity (Booth,
Clarke-Stewart, Vandell, McCartney, & Owen,
2002; Sf‌ifter, Coulehan, & Fish, 1993). In addi-
tion, several studies found that full-time use of
nonmaternal care was related to more sensitive
parenting of mothers (Burchinal, Bryant, Lee,
& Ramey, 1992; Crockenberg & Litman, 1991;
Schwartz, 1983), especially when the quality of
care was high (Caruso, 1989; Edwards, Logue,
Loehr, & Roth, 1986; Howes & Olenick, 1986).
These inconclusive f‌indings may be
attributable to methodological differences and
limitations. First, most studies used a small,
local sample. Some studies used samples of
mothers whose children were in a particular
type of care (e.g., high-quality center care;
Edwards et al., 1986); other studies focused on
middle-class families (e.g., Schwartz, 1983).
The variations in sample characteristics indicate
the possibility that the link between nonmaternal
care and maternal sensitivity may depend on
context. Second, most studies did not adequately
control for characteristics that were related
to both nonmaternal care characteristics and
maternal sensitivity, such as mothers’ parenting
values, leaving the question of a selection effect
unanswered.
Another limitation is overreliance on attach-
ment theory. Because it heavily focuses on the
negative inf‌luence of long hours of nonmaternal
care, attachment theory pays little attention to
other effects that nonmaternal care may have on
mothers’ parenting, such as the positive effects
of high-quality care found in prior research
(e.g., Burchinal et al., 1992). Also, it ignores
the possibility of variations in the inf‌luence of
nonmaternal care on mothers’ parenting that are
due to context (e.g., Clark et al., 1997). An
alternative perspective that expands the scope of
the link between nonmaternal care and mothers’
parenting is needed.
In this article, we address the conceptual
and methodological issues in prior research
by using a stress – resource perspective that
provides a broader theoretical scope and by
examining f‌ixed-effects models that take into
account unobserved characteristics. Below, we
discuss how a stress resource perspective
predicts the association between nonmaternal
care and mothers’ sensitive parenting and how
it further predicts the possibility of variations in
the link by levels of mothers’ resources. Then
we address the issue of selection effects.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT