More Is Not Always Better: The Case of Counterterrorism Security

Published date01 September 2019
Date01 September 2019
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1177/0022002718823924
Subject MatterArticles
Article
More Is Not Always
Better: The Case
of Counterterrorism
Security
Pritha Dev
1
, and Konrad Grabiszewski
2
Abstract
Can counterterrorism security be counterproductive? We argue that it can be when
the at-risk population acts strategically. We model a two-stage game where the
government first chooses the defensive security level for a public place. The second
stage is a simultaneous-move game with terrorist choosing terror effort and
members of the population deciding whether or not to attend the public place. Our
key measure of the efficiency of the counterterrorism security is the expected
number of casualties. Under very standard and general assumptions, we show that it
is possible that more security leads to an increase in that number. This is because
increasing security both discourages and encourages the terrorist. On the one hand,
more security makes a successful terror attack less likely (discouragement). On the
other hand, more security motivates more people to attend the public place which
makes the attack more valuable to the terrorist (encouragement).
Keywords
terrorism, counterterrorism security
1
Economics Area, Indian Institute of Management Ahmedabad, Ahmedabad, Gujarat, India
2
Prince Mohammad bin Salman College, Saudi Arabia
Corresponding Author:
Konrad Grabiszewski, Prince Mohammad bin Salman College, King Abdullah Economic City 23964, Saudi
Arabia.
Email: konrad.grabiszewski@gmail.com
Journal of Conflict Resolution
2019, Vol. 63(8) 1923-1938
ªThe Author(s) 2019
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/0022002718823924
journals.sagepub.com/home/jcr
According to the Oxford Engli sh Dictionar y, terrorism i s “the unlawful u se of
violence and intimidation, especially against civilians, in the pursuit of political
aims.”
1
Because of its violent nature and targeting civilian population, the fear of
terrorism is among the top worries of the modern world. In June 2017, 42 percent
of Americans were worried about being a victim of terrorism and 66 percent of
them consider the possibility of future terror attacks in the United States as a great
or fair concern (source: Gallup poll, http://news.gallup.com/poll/4909/terrorism-
united-states.aspx. Retrieved on January 12, 2018). In 2016, 79 perce nt of Amer-
ican respondents pointed at international terrorism as a critical threat to the United
States making it number one concern in the nation (development on nuclear weap-
ons by Iran was the second highest with 75 percent) (source: Gallup poll, http://
news.gallup.com/poll/189161/americans-cite-cyberterrorismamong-top-three-
threats.aspx. Retrieved on January 12, 2018).
Given that the fear of terrorism is widespread and significantly affects people’s
behavior, it is of utmost importance to incorporate it into the studies of terrorism.
The impact on behavior can be seen in a June 2017 poll, which shows that “as a
result of the events relatin g to terrorism in recent years,” 32 p ercent American
indicated that they are less willing to fly airplanes, 26 percent were less willing to
go to skyscrapers, 38 percent less willing to attend an event with thousands of
people, and 46 percent less willing to travel abroad (see Note 2).
Activities like going to a public place (e.g., 2008 Mumbai attacks), attending a
public event (e.g., 2017 Ariana Grande con cert in Manchester), or using public
transportation (e.g., 2004 Madrid train bombing) are associated with the ris k of
being targeted by terrorists. Whatever is “public” is a potential target of a terror
attack whose objective is to inflict a maximal loss of life. When deciding whether or
not to be part of a public event and go where expected crowd is to be, people take the
risk of terror attack into account and might opt out.
Our focus on “public places” is motivated by Brandt andSandler (2011) who show
that terroristsare now concentrating onattacking public places. Thereis also empirical
evidence which supports the fact that people change behavior as a response to terror.
Enders and Sandler(1991), Enders, Sandler, andParise (1992), and Drakos and Kutan
(2003) find evidenceof tourists not favoring destinations with higherrisk of terrorism.
Elias, Albert, andShiftan (2013) show that commutersin Israel choose private means
of transport over public transport as their risk perception of terror activities increase.
Kalist (2010) documents that in the United States, as the terror alert levels initially
went up after the September 2001 attacks, there was a dip in attendance for Major
League Baseball events. Abadie and Gardeazabal (2008) show how investors strate-
gically move away from countries and regions with higher terrorist risk. Sandler and
Enders (2008) provide an excellent review of economic consequences of terrorism.
In order to address the fear of terrorism and prevent terror attacks, governments
around the world spend large amounts of taxpayer money on defensive counter-
terrorism security.
2
For example, in the United States, that amount is around
1924 Journal of Conflict Resolution 63(8)

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT