A Link between Psychopathy Affect and Instrumentality in Homicide

AuthorJi Seun Sohn,Adrian Raine,Young-Oh Hong
Published date01 August 2022
Date01 August 2022
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1177/10887679211028879
Subject MatterArticles
https://doi.org/10.1177/10887679211028879
Homicide Studies
2022, Vol. 26(3) 308 –323
© 2021 SAGE Publications
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/10887679211028879
journals.sagepub.com/home/hsx
Article
A Link between Psychopathy
Affect and Instrumentality in
Homicide
Ji Seun Sohn1, Adrian Raine2,
and Young-Oh Hong3
Abstract
This study tests the hypothesis that psychopathy is more associated with instrumental
homicides than mixed and reactive homicides, and explores relationships between
Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) facet/item scores and different forms of
homicide: instrumental (n = 130), mixed (n = 103), and reactive (n = 219) homicides.
Instrumental homicides scored higher on facet 2 (p < .01) but scored lower on facet
4 (p < .1) compared to reactive homicides, whereas no facet scores differed between
mixed and reactive homicides. Among the items of facet 2 (affective), remorselessness
(item 6), and callousness (item 8) were predictive of instrumental homicide.
Keywords
PCL-R, instrumental/planned homicide, reactive/emotional homicide, mixture of
reactive and instrumental homicide, facet 2 (affective), lack of remorse or guilt (item 6),
callous/lack of empathy (item 8)
The psychopathy construct is a combination of interpersonal (facet 1), affective
(facet 2), lifestyle (facet 3), and antisocial (facet 4) features that have been often
utilized as one of the riskiest predictors for violent behaviors (Hare, 2003). Meta-
analyses have supported the view that psychopathy predicts institutional misbehav-
iors, nonviolent, and violent criminal behaviors as well as general recidivism (e.g.,
Guy et al., 2005; Kennealy et al., 2010; Leistico et al., 2008). A relationship between
1Georgia College & State University, Milledgeville, USA
2University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, USA
3Korean Institute of Criminology and Justice, Seoul, Republic of Korea
Corresponding Author:
Ji Seun Sohn, Department of Government and Sociology, Georgia College & State University, Campus
Box 18, Milledgeville, GA 31061-0490, USA.
Email: jiseun.sohn@gcsu.edu
1028879HSXXXX10.1177/10887679211028879Homicide StudiesSohn et al.
research-article2021
Sohn et al. 309
psychopathy and the instrumental-reactive distinction in violence research has also
received significant attention. While those with higher psychopathy were more
likely to be identified as instrumental offenders than those with lower psychopathy
(e.g., Cornell et al., 1996; Patrick, 2018; Raine, 1993), one meta-analysis questioned
the hypothesis that psychopathy is more related to instrumental compared to reactive
violence (Blais et al., 2014). Additionally, violent youths with high psychopathy
scores did not use significantly more instrumental violence than their counterparts
(Hutton & Woodworth, 2014). Moreover, a clear link between psychopathy and
homicide offending has been a focus of studies (e.g., Fox & DeLisi, 2019; Rodre
et al., 2019), and most of the homicides by psychopathic offenders were instrumen-
tal (Porter et al., 2018). However, no studies have assessed whether psychopathy
facets and items have relationships with instrumental/planned homicide that the cur-
rent study exclusively targeted.
Theories for Instrumental and Reactive Violence
The manifestations of different forms of violence and aggressive behavior vary. The
frustration-aggression hypothesis defines reactive aggression as the result of perceived
threat or frustration (Berkowitz, 1989). A systematic review suggested the attentional
bias theory in that hostile or threat-relevant stimuli (bias) is likely to trigger reactive
aggression (Manning, 2020). Correspondingly, hyperresponsivity to threat was identi-
fied as a risk factor for reactive aggression (Lickley & Sebastian, 2018). Regarding the
amygdala dysfunction hypothesis, while there is a strong link between psychopathy
and instrumental aggression, a link between psychopathy and reactive aggression was
not robust (Reidy et al., 2011). Such brain dysfunction can be central to understanding
the positive relationship between high sensitivity to alleged threats or frustrations and
reactive aggression (Bertsch et al., 2020). In contrast to reactive aggression, which is
perceived as more emotional, social learning theory views proactive aggression as
behavior that anticipates a reward (Bandura, 1973). Dehumanization theory explains
the psychological and social process of impersonalizing victims to impose instrumen-
tal violence (Markowitz & Slovic, 2020; Over, 2020). Thus, we would anticipate that
psychopathic individuals would be more likely to engage in instrumental and preda-
tory homicide relative to reactive homicide.
Comparisons between instrumental/proactive and emotion-laden/reactive violence
have been also reported. Violent offenders who were psychopathic used more instru-
mental violence without emotional arousal (Cornell et al., 1996), and proactive aggres-
sion was more linked with mothers’ rating of a psychopathic personality (Raine et al.,
2006). Although comparisons between instrumental and reactive violence are of value,
not all manifestations of violence are dichotomous. In one review, studies that catego-
rized proactive-only or reactive-only groups were relatively small, whereas many
studies have found reactive and proactive types of aggression to be more common
(Kempes et al., 2005). Consistently, homicides can be caused by humiliation, frustra-
tion, or rage as socialized emotions (reactive) or by an organized project (instrumen-
tal) (Katz, 1988), and psychopathic traits have been associated with both instrumental

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT