INCORPORATING ROUTINE ACTIVITIES, ACTIVITY SPACES, AND SITUATIONAL DEFINITIONS INTO THE SOCIAL SCHEMATIC THEORY OF CRIME

AuthorRONALD L. SIMONS,MAN‐KIT LEI,ERIC STEWART,CALLIE H. BURT,ASHLEY B. BARR
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/1745-9125.12053
Date01 November 2014
Published date01 November 2014
INCORPORATING ROUTINE ACTIVITIES, ACTIVITY
SPACES, AND SITUATIONAL DEFINITIONS INTO THE
SOCIAL SCHEMATIC THEORY OF CRIME
RONALD L. SIMONS,1CALLIE H. BURT,2ASHLEY B. BARR,3
MAN-KIT LEI,1and ERIC STEWART4
1Department of Sociology, University of Georgia
2School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Arizona State University
3Department of Sociology, University at Buffalo, SUNY
4School of Criminology, Florida State University
KEYWORDS: criminal propensity, neighborhood, action contexts, routine activities
Simons and Burt’s (2011) social schematic theory (SST) of crime posits that ad-
verse social factors are associated with offending because they promote a set of social
schemas (i.e., a criminogenic knowledge structure) that elevates the probability of sit-
uational definitions favorable to crime. This study extends the SST model by incorpo-
rating the role of contexts for action. Furthermore, the study advances tests of the SST
by incorporating a measure of criminogenic situational definitions to assess whether
such definitions mediate the effects of schemas and contexts on crime. Structural equa-
tion models using 10 years of panel data from 582 African American youth provided
strong support for the expanded theory. The results suggest that childhood and adoles-
cent social adversity fosters a criminogenic knowledge structure as well as selection into
criminogenic activity spaces and risky activities, all of which increase the likelihood of
offending largely through situational definitions. Additionally, evidence shows that the
criminogenic knowledge structure interacts with settings to amplify the likelihood of
situational definitions favorable to crime.
How do past experiences influence an individual’s propensity to offend? This key the-
oretical question drives criminological theorizing. Most theories of individual offending
attempt to identify the elements of criminal propensity and the mechanisms and processes
whereby past experiences give rise to these characteristics. In their recently developed so-
cial schematic theory (SST), Simons and Burt (2011) proposed social schemas to be the
Additional supporting information can be found in the listing for this article in the Wiley Online
Library at http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/crim.2014.52.issue-4/issuetoc.
Ronald L. Simons and Callie H. Burt contributed equally to this article. This research was sup-
ported by the National Institute of Mental Health (MH48165 and MH62669) and the Center for
Disease Control (029136–02). Additional funding for this project was provided by the National
Institute on Drug Abuse (DA021898 and 1P30DA027827) and the National Institute on Alco-
hol Abuse and Alcoholism (2R01AA012768 and 3R01AA012768–09S1). Direct correspondence
to Ronald L. Simons, Department of Sociology, University of Georgia, 324 Baldwin Hall, Athens,
GA 30602 (e-mail: rsimons@uga.edu).
C2014 American Society of Criminology doi: 10.1111/1745-9125.12053
CRIMINOLOGY Volume 52 Number 4 655–687 2014 655
656 SIMONS ET AL.
key theoretical mechanisms that account for the effect of past experiences on criminal
behavior.
SST emphasizes the role of several criminogenic social environments in shaping so-
cial schemas. In doing so, it integrates findings from a variety of traditions in criminol-
ogy that evince the importance of social adversity in shaping criminality, including those
related to neighborhood conditions, parenting, and racial discrimination (e.g., Agnew,
2006; Loeber and Farrington, 2000; Sampson and Laub, 1993; Tittle, 1995; Unnever and
Gabbidon, 2011). What unites these seemingly disparate social influences, according to
SST, is that all teach similar lessons about the future, social norms, and the nature of
people and relationships. As such, learning is central, and SST can be thought of as a
life-course learning theory. However, SST departs from the dominant learning theory in
criminology in several ways. Primary among these is its focus on the content of learn-
ing rather than on its form. Whereas Akers’s (1985) social learning theory emphasizes
operant learning principles, SST shifts the focus to the messages or tenets implicit in
the repeated patterns of interaction that occur in an individual’s social environment. Si-
mons and Burt (2011) argued that criminogenic conditions such as harsh parenting, racial
discrimination, and community disadvantage promote social schemas involving a hostile
view of people and relationships, a preference for immediate rewards, and a cynical view
of conventional norms. Furthermore, they posited that these three schemas are intercon-
nected and combine to form a criminogenic knowledge structure (CKS) that gives rise to
situational interpretations legitimating or compelling criminal and antisocial behavior.
In their initial test of the theory, Simons and Burt (2011) found strong support for the
SST model, as the identified social factors strongly influenced individuals’ social schemas,
which in turn increased the likelihood of offending. Indeed, with one exception, the effects
of all of the social factors they examined as well as of sex/gender and prior offending were
fully mediated by the CKS. Additional support for the theory was provided by Simons and
Barr (2012), who reported that much of the effect of romantic relationships on desistance
is explained by a reduction in the CKS. In addition, Burt and Simons (2013) showed that
racial discrimination increased the likelihood of offending through the CKS and that a
resilience factor, racial socialization, reduced offending through its effect on the CKS.
Thus, the initial support for SST is strong and promising. This work can be extended in
two clear ways. First, SST proposes that the CKS increases an individual’s probability of
engaging in crime by making it more likely that situations will be perceived as justifying
or requiring acts of law violation. As a result of data limitations, prior tests of SST were
unable to test the idea that the CKS influenced offending through definitions of the situ-
ation. With the addition of a measure of criminogenic situational definitions in the most
recent wave of the Family and Community Health Study (FACHS), we can test the idea
that criminogenic situational definitions are the mechanism through which CKS increases
the likelihood of offending. This is the first aim of the current study.
In addition, in their initial presentation of the theory, Simons and Burt (2011) focused
on the role of social environments as contexts for learning and development. As Wik-
str ¨
om et al. (2012; Wikstr ¨
om and Sampson, 2003) have noted, however, contexts are not
only sites for development but also sites for action. Individuals bring their social schemas
into various contexts, but schemas alone are not sufficient to motivate action. Actions,
including crime, result from the combination of individual characteristics and situational
cues. Moreover, individuals are not randomly placed in various contexts, but they ac-
tively seek out certain contexts consistent with their aims and preferences. Consistent with
CONTEXTS AND KNOWLEDGE STRUCTURES 657
recent work, rather than viewing selection as a nuisance in modeling, we view it as an im-
portant mechanism and causal force (e.g., Wikstr ¨
om et al., 2012; Sampson, 2012). Thus,
we examine whether individuals’ CKSs influence their likelihood of offending in part by
influencing the contexts in which they choose to spend their leisure time (selection). In
addition, we explore the idea that an individual’s CKS interacts with criminogenic con-
texts to amplify the likelihood of criminogenic situational definitions and, in turn, criminal
behavior. This idea, as will be elaborated on in the following section, is that individuals
with high CKSs are more likely to respond to situational inducements with crime than
those with lower criminal propensity.
In sum, the purpose of this article is both to elaborate SST and to test this elaboration in
a theory-sensitive research design. In doing so, we incorporate the role of social contexts
as sites both for development and for action into the theoretical model. In addition, we
test whether situational definitions serve as the mechanisms through which social schemas
and contexts influence criminal behavior. In the following pages, we discuss SST, focusing
extra attention on the elaborated role of context as a site for action, drawing especially on
insights and findings from situational action theory (Wikstr ¨
om et al., 2012), crime pattern
theory (Brantingham and Brantingham, 1984), and routine activities theory (Cohen and
Felson, 1979). We then test this model using several waves of panel data from a sample
of several hundred African American young adults from the FACHS. Given its inclusion
of measures of both developmental and interactional contexts, as well as a host of other
strengths including its longitudinal design and measures designed to test SST, the FACHS
is particularly well suited for evaluating the elaborated SST model under consideration.
SOCIAL SCHEMAS AND SITUATIONAL DEFINITIONS
SST starts with the assumption that humans adapt to survive in their environments,
and a significant part of this adaptation is cognitive. The theory assumes, consistent with a
burgeoning body of work on human morality, that humans are born with innate capacities
to be fair, cooperative, and sympathetic (e.g., Alexander, 1987; De Waal, 2006; Hauser,
2006) as well as to be selfish, egoistic, and sometimes aggressive (Shermer, 2004; Smith,
2007). Rather than being born good, bad, or as empty vessels into which society pours
its views of morality, SST assumes that we are born with the capacity (i.e., the wiring) to
adapt our orientation to fit our environment. Humans have evolved to survive in a variety
of contexts, which vary in the degree to which they are supportive and predictable versus
hostile and dangerous and, thus, require different orientations and competencies (Belsky,
Scholomer, and Ellis, 2012; Ellis et al., 2012). The emphasis is on the fact that individuals
adapt to survive, not necessarily to thrive, in the contexts in which they find themselves
and that egoistic, unkind, and criminal behavior can be incited by such adaptations. Given
these assumptions, the theory’s focus is on the role of social environments—especially
persistent and memorable ones—in blunting humans’ innate capacity to be sympathetic,
fair, and cooperative into an orientation that is cautious, self-defensive, selfish, and even
hostile.
From this perspective, offenders do not engage in criminal behavior despite their
“morality” or commitment to conventional norms. Rather, individuals offend because
their interpretations of situations shaped by past experiences lead them to believe that
criminal acts are required or justified by the exigencies of the situation. This perspective
is supported by evidence that most individuals do not believe that their illegal actions

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT