Immunity to Environmental Crime, Harm and Violence: An Ongoing Pandemic and a Possible Narrative Vaccine

AuthorAvi Brisman
Published date01 March 2022
DOI10.1177/0306624X20970885
Date01 March 2022
Subject MatterArticles
https://doi.org/10.1177/0306624X20970885
International Journal of
Offender Therapy and
Comparative Criminology
2022, Vol. 66(4) 451 –469
© The Author(s) 2020
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/0306624X20970885
journals.sagepub.com/home/ijo
Article
Immunity to Environmental
Crime, Harm and Violence:
An Ongoing Pandemic and a
Possible Narrative Vaccine
Avi Brisman1,2,3
Abstract
As of June 2020, there have been at least 2,540 mass shootings since the Sandy
Hook Elementary School shooting in Newtown, CT, on December 14, 2012. Some
have suggested that the repeated trauma of these massacres has created a collective
“emotional numbness,” lessening our empathy. This article asks whether a similar
phenomenon is occurring with respect to environmental crime and harm. It considers
whether we have developed “compassion fatigue” regarding environmental violence
and contemplates a “workout regimen” for empathy for Gaia’s suffering. In so doing,
it seeks to engage with emerging work in the penumbra of narrative criminology and
green cultural criminology.
Keywords
climate change, compassion fatigue, empathy, environmental crime, environmental
harm, green cultural criminology, green criminology, narratives/stories, pandemic,
vaccine
Introduction
This article begins with a number of propositions or statements, which I put forth in
order to set up a comparison between so-called “violent crimes” (and homicides, in
particular) and “environmental crimes.”
1Eastern Kentucky University, Richmond, KY, USA
2Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
3University of Newcastle, Callaghan, NSW, Australia
Corresponding Author:
Avi Brisman, School of Justice Studies College of Justice and Safety Eastern Kentucky University, 521
Lancaster Avenue, 467 Stratton Richmond, KY 40475 USA.
Email: avi.brisman@eku.edu
970885IJOXXX10.1177/0306624X20970885International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative CriminologyBrisman
research-article2020
452 International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology 66(4)
First, “many people never will be directly affected by crime” and most people are
never the (direct) victim of a mass shooting (Schildkraut et al., 2018, pp. 224, 223).1
In contrast, many more people are direct victims of environmental harms (see Lynch,
2013). To be sure, all of us are, in some way, victims, as well as offenders, in the crime
of climate change. Indeed, “the impacts of climate change have and will (or, at least,
pose the potential to) ‘penetrate all aspects of environment, society and economy’
(Schipper & Pelling, 2006, pp. 27–28)” (Brisman, 2015, p. 182).
Second, “individual beliefs and perceptions of crime typically are shaped by the
media (Chermak, 1994; Graber, 1980; Maguire et al., 1999; Mayr & Machin, 2012;
Pollak & Kubrin, 2007; Robinson, 2011; Surette, 1992; Warr, 2000). Studies have
shown that the mass media are the primary source of information about crime for
nearly 95% of the general population (Graber, 1980; Surette, 1992)” (Schildkraut
et al., 2018, p. 224). Moreover, Schildkraut et al. (2018, p. 223) stress that the way in
which mass shootings are understood is shaped almost solely by the mass media.
Unfortunately, as Ozymy et al. (2020, p. 150) point out, “it is still the case that the
general public is unaware of the presence of most green crimes unless there is wide-
spread media attention and mass victimization (Jarrell, 2009).” In fact, “people who
experience personally the consequences of environmental problems are more likely to
express concern (see Running et al., 2017)” than those who do not (Brisman, 2018,
p. 471). But, as Renkl (2019) reminds us, “just because we can’t see something”—and,
we might add, just because we do not experience something personally—“doesn’t
mean it isn’t happening.”2 Indeed, in earlier work (Brisman, 2014), I considered
whether we might suffer from micropsia, myopia or some other ophthalmological ail-
ment with respect to climate change. In that piece, I referred to climate change as an
“invisible crime” on the grounds that while we may intellectually grasp the idea of
climate change and have some experience of climate change—or some experience that
we attribute to climate change—we do not see climate change or feel it the way we
might other discrete environmental “events,” and thus the lag time between emissions
and their effects on climate makes it especially difficult to mobilize political will to
address the problem.3 Likewise, Per Espen Stoknes, a Norwegian psychologist who is
serving a term in the Parliament of Norway, points to the “distancing barrier” that
obstructs people’s focus on climate change and its solutions. As Stoknes explains, “[w]
hen we speak about climate, it’s far out into the future, usually in 2050 or beyond. CO2
is invisible, climate issues are happening somewhere else, and somebody else is
responsible. I’m sure if climate change were a bad-smelling, brownish haze that some
tyrant, lunatic, or crook was releasing into the world, we’d join together and shoot him
out of existence. But climate change is invisible, slow-moving, and doesn’t smell; and
if there’s an enemy, it’s us” (quoted in Suttie, 2018).
Third, research tends to indicate that news coverage is skewed toward stories about
crime (Schildkraut et al., 2018, pp. 223–224 (citing Chermak, 1995; Graber, 1980;
Maguire et al, 1999; Surette, 1992)). Violent crimes—and homicides, in particular—
often are “the most newsworthy because they have the ability to capture and keep the
audience’s attention, even though property offenses are considerably more common
(Chermak, 1995; Gruenewald et al., 2009; Meyers, 1997; Paulsen, 2003; Pritchard,

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT