How Moral Beliefs Influence Collective Violence. Evidence From Lynching in Mexico

Published date01 January 2025
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1177/00104140231223747
AuthorEnzo Nussio
Date01 January 2025
Article
Comparative Political Studies
2025, Vol. 58(1) 4377
© The Author(s) 2023
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/00104140231223747
journals.sagepub.com/home/cps
How Moral Beliefs
Inf‌luence Collective
Violence. Evidence From
Lynching in Mexico
Enzo Nussio
Abstract
How do moral beliefs inf‌luence favorability to collective violence? In this
article, I argue that, f‌irst, moral beliefs are inf‌luential depending on their
salience, as harm avoidance is a common moral concern. The more accessible
moral beliefs in decision-making, the more they restrain harmful behavior.
Second, moral beliefs are inf‌luential depending on their content. Group-
oriented moral beliefs can overturn the harm avoidance principle and
motivate individuals to favor collective violence. Analysis is based on a
representative survey in Mexico City and focuses on a proximate form of
collective violence, locally called lynching. Findings support both logics of
moral inf‌luence. Experimentally induced moral salience reduces favorability to
lynching, and group-oriented moral beliefs are related to more favorability.
Against existing theories that downplay the relevance of morality and present
it as cheap talk, these f‌indings demonstrate how moral beliefs can both re-
strain and motivate collective violence.
Keywords
morality, harm avoidance, collective violence, emotions, survey, Mexico
ETH Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
Corresponding Author:
Enzo Nussio, ETH Zurich, Haldeneggsteig 4, Zurich 8092, Switzerland.
Email: enzo.nussio@sipo.gess.ethz.ch
Data Availability Statement included at the end of the article
Introduction
Collective violence is often justif‌ied with moral claims. The military invasion
of a country, for example, is framed as an act of self-defense, rioters derive
their legitimacy from perceived injustice, and mobs attack thieves to provide
deserved punishment. Appealing to a greater good, such as protection,justice,
and punishment, makes the inf‌liction of harm look necessary (Fiske & Rai,
2014;Ginges, 2019). These examples show that morality plays a key role for
collective violence. After all, morality concerns our beliefs about right and
wrong behavior, including the acceptable use of violence (Dill & Schubiger,
2021). However, political scientists have rarely studied the effects of moral
beliefs on violence. They rather examine morality as an attribute of violence,
focusing on the conditions under which violence is perceived to be moral or
legitimate as empirical political scientists have done (Dill & Schubiger,
2021;VandenBerg, 2021), or focusing on the normative conditions under
which war is moral or justif‌ied as political theorists have done (Bellamy,
2006; e.g. Lazar, 2017).
In this study, I ask how the moral beliefs individuals hold inf‌luence their
favorability to collective violence. I thus focus on morality as a driver of
violence rather than an attribute of violence. Despite its central importance,
this question has received surprisingly little attention. Why? On the one hand,
moral beliefs are an intricate aspect of dominant theories on collective vio-
lence and are thus hard to separate from other key concepts. Theories focusing
on identity (Horowitz, 1985), grievances (Cederman et al., 2013), culture
(Huntington, 2011), ideology (Maynard, 2019), and religion (Fox, 2004)
subsume morality under a broader category. Identity scholars, for instance,
often refer to clashing moral worldviews of rival groups; scholars of religion
see religious beliefs as source of morality; and grievance scholars make
important assumptions about injustice and group loyalty. However, they do
not intend to disentangle moral aspects from these broader categories.
On the other hand, moral beliefs are an exceptionally challenging object for
empirical inquiry. Some scholars claim that moral beliefs represent merely
post-hoc justif‌ication with no explanatory weight for behavior (Swidler, 1986;
Walter, 2017). According to this view, expressing moral concerns to justify
violent action is cheap talk. Others argue that moral beliefs are a spurious
factor. Given that morality is an adaptation to social context, resulting be-
havior is determined by precisely that context and not morality (Tilly, 2003).
Yet another group of scholars views moral beliefs as a consequence of vi-
olence rather than a cause of it (Solomon et al., 1991;Staub, 1999). These
empirical challenges in part explain why students of collective violence have
eschewed moral beliefs.
With the present study, I focus on the moral beliefs held by individuals.
They harbor an important explanation for collective violence, as collective
44 Comparative Political Studies 58(1)
violence often faces problems of collective action, given the risks implied in
personal participation (Wood, 2003). Moral beliefs are one candidate that can
help individuals overcome such barriers. The f‌irst contribution of this study is
theoretical: I offer a theory of how moral beliefs can inf‌luence favorability to
collective violence, distinct from theories about identity, grievances, and other
dominant theories. This studysf‌indings are thus relevant for theories on
collective violence that embed aspects of morality. The second contribution is
empirical: I devise a strategy that overcomes the profound challenges to
empirical inquiry, by comparing individuals living under the same contextual
conditions and using experimentation to discard post-hoc justif‌ication and
reverse causation.
Against existing theories that downplay the relevance of morality or
subsume it under broader categories, I argue that moral beliefs can inf‌luence
an individuals favorability to collective violence through two distinct logics:
moral salience and group-oriented moral beliefs. First, moral beliefs are
inf‌luential if they are a salient ingredient of decision-making. Decision-
making draws on different types of beliefs as input (Ajzen, 1991). When
moral beliefs are salient, meaning when they are on ones mind, they should
act like a brake on violence, given that harm avoidance is a common some
argue universal moral concern. Mainstream psychology, in fact, views
morality as a musclethat restrains harmful behavior (Baumeister, 1999).
Bandura (2016) even argues that to inf‌lict harm, a person needs to morally
disengage, suggesting that morality can be switched either onor off. Authors
focusing on such restraint, a key insight in moral disengagement theory, tend
to see the moral domain as limited to universal values of harm avoidance and
fairness. In line with this thinking, I argue that moral salience reduces fa-
vorability to collective violence. Observing such an empirical pattern would
challenge rationalist and materialist theories, which make us believe that
moral concerns have no bearing on violence.
Second, and distinct from the logic of moral salience, the content of moral
beliefs can also be inf‌luential. In contradiction to claims to moral universalism
(Kohlberg, 1981), which are baked into the moral salience logic, moral beliefs
vary across societies, communities, and individuals, ref‌lecting moral plu-
ralism (Graham et al., 2011). This is an important insight often attributed to
anthropologist Richard Shweder et al. (1997), but noted much earlier for
example by Edward Westermarck (1908). This variation allows us to examine
how certain kinds of moral beliefs inf‌luence violence. One of the most
universally held moral principles is avoidance of harm, which should reduce
favorability to violence, in accordance with the logic of moral salience.
However, overriding moral goods, especially if they relate to groups, can
motivate individuals to inf‌lict harm (Fiske & Rai, 2014;Hoover et al., 2021).
Hence, I argue that group-oriented moral beliefs, stressing collective values
such as loyalty and authority more than individual goods, increase favorability
Nussio 45

Get this document and AI-powered insights with a free trial of vLex and Vincent AI

Get Started for Free

Start Your 3-day Free Trial of vLex and Vincent AI, Your Precision-Engineered Legal Assistant

  • Access comprehensive legal content with no limitations across vLex's unparalleled global legal database

  • Build stronger arguments with verified citations and CERT citator that tracks case history and precedential strength

  • Transform your legal research from hours to minutes with Vincent AI's intelligent search and analysis capabilities

  • Elevate your practice by focusing your expertise where it matters most while Vincent handles the heavy lifting

vLex

Start Your 3-day Free Trial of vLex and Vincent AI, Your Precision-Engineered Legal Assistant

  • Access comprehensive legal content with no limitations across vLex's unparalleled global legal database

  • Build stronger arguments with verified citations and CERT citator that tracks case history and precedential strength

  • Transform your legal research from hours to minutes with Vincent AI's intelligent search and analysis capabilities

  • Elevate your practice by focusing your expertise where it matters most while Vincent handles the heavy lifting

vLex

Start Your 3-day Free Trial of vLex and Vincent AI, Your Precision-Engineered Legal Assistant

  • Access comprehensive legal content with no limitations across vLex's unparalleled global legal database

  • Build stronger arguments with verified citations and CERT citator that tracks case history and precedential strength

  • Transform your legal research from hours to minutes with Vincent AI's intelligent search and analysis capabilities

  • Elevate your practice by focusing your expertise where it matters most while Vincent handles the heavy lifting

vLex

Start Your 3-day Free Trial of vLex and Vincent AI, Your Precision-Engineered Legal Assistant

  • Access comprehensive legal content with no limitations across vLex's unparalleled global legal database

  • Build stronger arguments with verified citations and CERT citator that tracks case history and precedential strength

  • Transform your legal research from hours to minutes with Vincent AI's intelligent search and analysis capabilities

  • Elevate your practice by focusing your expertise where it matters most while Vincent handles the heavy lifting

vLex

Start Your 3-day Free Trial of vLex and Vincent AI, Your Precision-Engineered Legal Assistant

  • Access comprehensive legal content with no limitations across vLex's unparalleled global legal database

  • Build stronger arguments with verified citations and CERT citator that tracks case history and precedential strength

  • Transform your legal research from hours to minutes with Vincent AI's intelligent search and analysis capabilities

  • Elevate your practice by focusing your expertise where it matters most while Vincent handles the heavy lifting

vLex

Start Your 3-day Free Trial of vLex and Vincent AI, Your Precision-Engineered Legal Assistant

  • Access comprehensive legal content with no limitations across vLex's unparalleled global legal database

  • Build stronger arguments with verified citations and CERT citator that tracks case history and precedential strength

  • Transform your legal research from hours to minutes with Vincent AI's intelligent search and analysis capabilities

  • Elevate your practice by focusing your expertise where it matters most while Vincent handles the heavy lifting

vLex

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT